Review
Abstract
Background: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is relatively common among school-age children. Technology-based interventions, such as computer-assisted training programs, neurofeedback training, and virtual reality, show promise in regulating the behaviors and cognitive functions of children with ADHD. An increasing number of randomized controlled trials have been conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of these technologies in improving the conditions of children with ADHD.
Objective: This study aims to conduct a systematic review of technological interventions for school-age children with ADHD and perform a meta-analysis of the outcomes of technology-based interventions.
Methods: A total of 19 randomized controlled studies involving 1843 participants were selected from a pool of 2404 articles across 7 electronic databases spanning from their inception to April 2022. ADHD behaviors, cognitive functions, learning ability, and quality of life were addressed in this study.
Results: Random effects meta-analyses found that children with ADHD receiving technology-based intervention showed small and significant effect sizes in computer-rated inattention (standardized mean difference [SMD] −0.35; P<.04), parent-rated overall executive function measured by the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (SMD −0.35; P<.04), parent-rated disruptive behavior disorder measured by the Child Behavior Checklist (SMD −0.50; P<.001) and Disruptive Behavior Disorder Rating Scale (SMD −0.31; P<.02), and computer-rated visual attention measured by the Continuous Performance Test (SMD −0.42; P<.001) and Reaction Time (SMD −0.43; P<.02).
Conclusions: Technology-based interventions are promising treatments for improving certain ADHD behaviors and cognitive functions among school-age children with ADHD.
Trial Registration: PROSPERO CRD42023446924; https://tinyurl.com/7ee5t24n
doi:10.2196/51459
Keywords
Introduction
Background
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders among school-age children [
], with a prevalence of 7.6% [ ]. Meanwhile, according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Text Revision Fourth Edition [ ], the prevalence of ADHD in school-age children is 3% to 7% [ ]. The 3 main categories of ADHD symptoms are inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, which usually manifest in the school-age period [ ]. These symptoms have detrimental impacts on the quality of life and functioning, including self-esteem, academic performance, social functioning, and relationship building [ ]. ADHD is usually associated with long-term disability [ ]. The types of treatments vary in different stages of life [ ]. Behavioral parenting training and medication are the common approaches used to improve the behaviors and self-control of school-age children with ADHD [ ]. ADHD medications are associated with an increased risk of headache, anxiety, and sleep disturbances [ , ]. Behavioral therapies are generally limited by time and space [ ]. Therefore, feasible nonpharmacological approaches are recommended as alternatives to regulate the behaviors, executive functions, and well-being of children with ADHD.In the last 10 years, human-computer interaction has widely been recognized in psychiatric and mental health research [
, ]. Digital technologies, neurofeedback systems, and virtual reality for health support, care, and treatment have increasingly been adopted, thus successfully gaining psychological health advantage [ , ]. ADHD is one of the common psychiatric disorders for which technology-based treatments are often used as therapeutic tools [ , ]. The application and effectiveness of technology-based interventions have been evaluated in ADHD treatment [ , , ]. The advantages of technology-based treatment include improved executive functions and increased physiological and mental well-being [ , ]. However, inconsistent results have been reported regarding the efficiency of technology-based intervention in school-age children with ADHD. Regarding ADHD behaviors, Dovis et al [ ], Egeland et al [ ], Steiner et al [ ], and van der Oord et al [ ] found that computer-based training improved the inattention and hyperactivity in children with ADHD. However, some studies did not find significant results between technological treatment and ADHD behaviors [ - ]. Regarding executive functions, technology-based treatments improved inhibition [ ], working memory [ , ], flexibility [ , ], emotional control [ ], initiation [ ], planning and organization [ ], organizing materials [ ], monitoring [ ], and metacognition [ , ]. However, several studies determined that technology-based interventions have no effect on executive functions [ , , ]. Regarding disruptive behavior disorder, Dovis et al [ ], Lim et al [ ], Steiner et al [ ], and van der Oord et al [ ] reported that computer-assisted training and neurofeedback training regulated oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder, whereas Bikic et al [ ] and Breider et al [ ] found no significant effect. Some studies have discovered that technology-based interventions can significantly improve visual attention [ , , ], yet numerous studies indicated no significant effects [ , , , , ]. These contradictory results make it difficult to examine the effectiveness of technology-based interventions in school-age children with ADHD.Previous reviews illustrated the effectiveness of different types of technologies on children with ADHD [
- ]. Cibrian et al [ ] and Powell et al [ ] summarized this topic through a narrative description. Although Powell et al [ ] adopted a meta-analysis to synthesize previous studies, their research focused on the use of virtual reality among children and adolescents with ADHD.Objective
These existing studies based on nonrandomized, cross-sectional, and observational designs have added to the knowledge base and identified the potential implications of technology in enhancing the ability and functions of children. In addition, more randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have been conducted in the recent decade, as a growing number of researchers have shown interest in the use of technology-based interventions to improve the capability and well-being of children with ADHD. A meta-analysis of RCTs can provide strong and robust evidence regarding the effectiveness of technology-based interventions in improving children with ADHD. Therefore, a systematic review and meta-analysis of the existing evidence of RCTs are needed to explicate the advantages of technology to school-age children with ADHD.
Methods
Search Strategy
This review focused on RCTs using technologies to regulate the ability and alleviate the well-being of children with ADHD. This review was registered in the PROSPERO International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (registration number CRD42023446924). The studies included in this review were searched from electronic databases, including PubMed, MEDLINE, ScienceDirect, Web of Science, CINAHL (via EBSCO), PsycINFO (via OVID), and Scopus, in April 2022. The keywords used in the search engines were as follows:
- Population: “children with ADHD” or “school-age children with ADHD” or “students with ADHD”
- Intervention: “technology” or “computer” or “robots” or “virtual reality” or “VR” or “augmented reality” or “AR” or “web-based” or “serious games”
- Outcomes: “inattentive” or “hyperactive-impulsive” or “hyperactivity” or “impulsivity” or “executive functions” or “executive functioning” or “inhibition” or “working memory” or “emotional control” or “flexibility” or “attention” or “initiation” or “planning” or “organization” or “time management” or “metacognition” or “quality of life” or “performance”
shows the full search strategy.
Eligibility Criteria
The titles and abstracts of the selected papers were screened to identify relevant papers for this review. The analysis was performed using Population, Intervention, Control, and Outcomes framework (1) population: patients who were diagnosed with ADHD and aged between 6 and 12 years; (2) intervention: using technology (ie, the method of applying scientific knowledge for practical purposes) without restriction of technology type or frequency of intervention; (3) comparison: technology-based interventions for managing ADHD versus no interventions, interventions with placebo effect, and treatment as usual or waitlist control; and (4) outcome: behaviors, cognitive functions, and whole well-being evaluation results for patients with ADHD. Studies were selected using the following inclusion criteria: (1) articles published in English and (2) RCTs. Exclusion criteria included (1) teenagers, adults, and older adults; (2) comorbidity with autism spectrum disorder, psychosis, and affective or anxiety disorder; (3) consumption of toxic substances; (4) diagnosed with learning disorder; (5) non–peer reviewed studies; and (6) qualitative studies, reviews, cross-sectional studies, case studies, observational studies, study protocols, pre-post studies without a control group, or conference abstracts without full text. Furthermore, the references of the included papers were manually checked for eligibility. After removing duplicate articles, the studies were independently screened by 2 reviewers (KPW and BZ). The selected full-text articles were retrieved and reviewed by 4 reviewers (KPW, JQ, YJX, and BZ).
Quality Assessment
The risk of bias in each study were independently evaluated by 3 reviewers (KPW, JQ, and BZ) using the Cochrane Collaboration tool for assessing the risk of bias [
]. The PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) criteria were adopted for conducting this review [ ]. demonstrates the PRISMA checklist. The criteria of the tool included (1) random sequence generation, (2) allocation concealment, (3) blinding of participants and personnel, (4) blinding of outcome assessment, (5) incomplete outcome data, (6) selective reporting, and (7) other bias. In addition, judgment has 3 levels, including “low risk of bias,” “high risk of bias,” and “unclear risk of bias.” The conflicting results were settled by 4 reviewers (KPW, JQ, YJX, and BZ) through discussion.Data Extraction
The information of the selected studies was extracted and coded into different categories, including study characteristics (first author’s name, publication year, country, and setting), characteristics of participants (sample size, sex, and age), intervention and control condition (type of technology used, frequency, length, and duration), outcome measurement (rating scale, test, and questionnaire), and result (mean and SD).
Data Synthesis
Data processing and analysis were conducted using the Review Manager Software RevMan (version 5.4), Cochrane Collaboration. The standardized mean difference (SMD) with a 95% CI was used to compute the effect size of the continuous outcomes of the interventions. The mean value of the baseline and posttest with SDs and the number of participants in the intervention groups and control groups were selected for the effect size calculation (ie, effect size of group differences). If reported, we selected the results estimated by the analysis of covariance, which treats individual baseline scores as covariates to correct for regression to baseline imbalanced means [
]. If the analysis of covariance had not been reported, change from baseline with SDs and posttest with SDs were selected. Multiple effect sizes were included in the same study, which contradicted the assumption that the effect sizes are independent of each other in the conventional meta-analytic procedures because the effect sizes in one study may be more correlated than those in other studies [ ]. The results would become biased if this dependency was not considered. Hence, effect sizes assessed by the same measures were clustered to estimate the association between technology-based interventions and the conditions of school-age children with ADHD. The random effects model was applied in the meta-analysis, given the methodological diversity across the studies. Heterogeneity was measured using the I2 value (P<.10; I2>50%), and the higher the value of I2, the higher the level of heterogeneity. Subgroup analysis of parent-rated, teacher-rated, computer-rated, and self-rated results was performed, where applicable. To test moderating effects, 5 study-level characteristics, including the number of sessions, sample size, setting, game elements, and types of control group, were selected to calculate the meta-regression for each moderator.Results
Selected Articles
A total of 2404 articles were retrieved from electronic databases. After removing duplicates, the titles and abstracts of 1568 articles were screened. In total, 1347 articles were excluded, and the remaining 221 articles were selected for full-text screening. At this stage, we excluded reviews (n=65), experimental studies (n=31), cross-sectional studies (n=21), longitudinal studies (n=25), qualitative studies (n=24), case studies (n=19), mixed methods studies (n=8), study protocols (n=4), RCT registrations (n=3), and non-English publications (n=2). Finally, 19 RCTs that met the inclusion and exclusion criteria were identified. A flowchart of the study selection process is shown in
.Risk of Bias
A low risk of random sequence generation was recorded from 8 trials (ie, the use of a computer random number generator and web-based system). Seven studies reported a low risk of allocation concealment. Meanwhile, 18 studies had a low risk of blinding participants and personnel. Although some studies had no complete blinding of participants and personnel [
- , , , , , ], the reviewers determined that the outcomes were not likely to be affected by a lack of blinding. Six studies were judged to have a low risk of blinding the outcome assessment. Seven studies indicated no blinding of assessors, and 6 studies did not clearly indicate the blinding of assessors. All studies were judged to have a low risk of incomplete outcome data because the attrition rate of all studies was <20%. All studies were judged to have a low risk of selective reporting, as nearly all studies had the protocol and all studies reported the primary and secondary outcomes. Ten studies described the way to manage missing data (ie, intention-to-treat analysis) [ , - , , , , , ]. The risk of bias assessment is shown in and [ , - , , , , , ].Study Characteristics
Fourteen studies used a 2-arm trials design [
- , - , - ], and the remaining 5 studies had a 3-arm trial design [ , , , , ]. These studies were conducted in 11 countries: Canada [ ], Denmark [ ], France [ ], Germany [ , ], Iran [ ], the Netherlands [ , , ], Norway [ ], Singapore [ ], Spain [ , ], Switzerland [ ], and the United States [ , , , , ].Participants Characteristics
A total of 1843 school-age children were included in this review, all of whom were diagnosed with ADHD. The number of participants included in each trial ranged from 10 to 246. Most studies were conducted in participants’ homes, and a small number of studies were conducted in schools, classrooms, and clinics. Detailed information is presented in
.Study | Country | Setting | Sample size, n | Sex (female/male), n/n | Age (years), mean (range)a |
Azami et al [ | ]Iran | Participants’ home | 34 | 0/34 | 7-12 |
Benzing and Schmidt [ | ]Switzerland | Participants’ home | 51 | 10/41 | 10.43 (8-12) |
Bigorra et al [ | ]Spain | Participants’ home | 66 | 36/24 | 8.9 |
Bikic et al [ | ]Denmark | Participants’ home | 70 | 11/59 | 9.96 |
Bioulac et al [ | ]France | No mention | 51 | 10/41 | 8.9 |
Breider et al [ | ]Netherlands | Participants’ home | 21 | 6/15 | 7.76 |
Corkum et al [ | ]Canada | Classroom-based | 58 | 7/51 | 8.83 (6-12) |
Dovis et al [ | ]Netherlands | Home-based training | 89 | 18/71 | 10.50 |
Egeland et al [ | ]Norway | School | 67 | 18/49 | 10.4 |
Fried et al [ | ]The United States | Participants’ home | 333 | 88/245 | 9.13 |
Gevensleben et al [ | ]Germany | No mention | 94 | 17/77 | 9.21 |
Kofler et al [ | ]The United States | Office and participants’ home | 54 | 12/42 | 10.41 |
Kollins et al [ | ]The United States | Participants’ home | 348 | 100/248 | 9.65 |
Lim et al [ | ]Singapore | Clinic | 172 | 25/147 | 8.6 |
Meyer et al [ | ]The United States | Home | 40 | 12/28 | 10.15 |
Moreno-García et al [ | ]Spain | Home and school | 57 | 13/44 | 8.84 |
Steiner et al [ | ]The United States | Classroom | 104 | 34/70 | 8.56 |
van der Oord et al [ | ]Netherlands | Home | 40 | 7/33 | 9.75 |
Wangler et al [ | ]Germany | No mention | 94 | 17/77 | 9.64 |
aIf studies did not provide mean values and/or ranges for participant ages, this information was not shown in the table.
Technology-Based Intervention and Control Condition
Different types of technologies were adopted in the intervention group among the included studies, including computer-assisted training programs, neurofeedback training, and virtual reality. For the control groups, 6 studies used treatment or medication as the usual approach. The participants in the control groups of 3 included studies did not receive any training. Two studies used stimulants, 7 studies used placebo cognitive training, and 1 study used a digital game. Detailed information is presented in
.Study | Intervention | Control | Type of intervention | Outcome measurement |
Azami et al [ | ]Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation | Psychostimulants | Individual: 20 sessions in 3 mo, 90 min/session | Continuous Performance Test, Tower-of-London, Forward/Backward Digit Span From WISC-Ra, Raven Progressive Matrices, and web-based version of Span Board Task Progressive Matrices |
Benzing and Schmidt [ | ]Exergame training | Not receiving training | Individual: 8 wk, 3 times a week for at least 30 min | Conners‐3 Rating Scales and German Motor Test |
Bigorra et al [ | ]Computerized working memory training | Nonadaptive work memory training | Individual: 5 wk, 5 sessions per week, 30-45 min/session | Backward Digit Span, Letter-number Sequencing of WISC-IVb, Backward Spatial Span of WMS-IIIc, Lowa Gambling Task, Happé Strange Stories, and Folk Psychology Test |
Bikic et al [ | ]Cognitive computer games of the ACTIVATE program | Treatment as usual | Individual: 6 times a week, 8 wk | Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, Motor Screening Task, Attention Switching Task, Rapid Visual Information Processing, Intraextra Dimensional Set Shift, Reaction Time, ADHDd Rating Scale, and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function |
Bioulac et al [ | ]Virtual classroom cognitive remediation program | Psychotherapy placebo training | Individual: twice a week for 6 wk, 12 sessions, 30 min/session | ADHD Rating Scale and Continuous Performance Test Task Assessment |
Breider et al [ | ]Web-based program and supportive therapist contact | Face-to-face parent training | Parents: 17 sessions, 45-60 min/session | Child Behavior Checklist |
Corkum et al [ | ]Web-based learning and blackboard learning | Treatment as usual | Teachers: 6 training sessions | Conners-3 Parent and Teacher Rating Scales and Impairment Rating Scale |
Dovis et al [ | ]Braingame Brian training | Placebo-mode working memory training | Individual: 25 training sessions in 5 wk, 35-50 min per session | Disruptive Behavior Disorder Rating Scale, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire for Children, Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory, and Home Situations Questionnaire |
Egeland et al [ | ]Cogmed RoboMemo program | Treatment as usual | Individual: daily basis for 5-7 wk, 30-45 min | Color Word Test, Trail Making Test, Conners Continuous Performance Test-II, Key Math, Logometrica, Benton Visual Retention Test, Children’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test-2, ADHD Rating Scale, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function |
Fried et al [ | ]Text messaging | Treatment as usual | Individual: 45 d | Adherence to stimulants |
Gevensleben et al [ | ]Neurofeedback training | Attention skills training | Individual: 6 mo, 25-30 min/session | ADHD Rating Scale, German Rating Scale for Oppositional Defiant/Conduct Disorders, Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire, Home Situations Questionnaire, and Homework Problem Checklist |
Kofler et al [ | ]Central executive training | Inhibitory control training | Group: 10 wk, 1 h/wk; individual: 10 wk, 2-3 d/wk, 15 min/d | Behavior Assessment System for Children, ADHD Rating Scale, and Phonological and Visuospatial Reordering |
Kollins et al [ | ]AKL-T01 (a digital therapeutic) | Digital game | Individual: 5 d/wk for 4 wk, 25 min/d | ADHD Rating Scale, Test of Variables of Attention, and Attention Performance Index |
Lim et al [ | ]Brain computer interface-based attention training program | Not receiving training | Individual: 3 sessions/wk in the first 8 wk and 4 sessions/wk in the next 12 wk | ADHD Rating Scale Inattention Score and Child Behavior Checklist |
Meyer et al [ | ]Computerized training | Medication as usual | Individual: at least 5 d a week for 4 wk, 15 min/d | Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham-IV Questionnaire and Conners Parent Rating Scale and Conners Teacher Rating Scale |
Moreno-García et al [ | ]Neurofeedback training | Pharmacology | Individual: 40 sessions | ADHD Rating Scale and Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test |
Steiner et al [ | ]Computer attention training using neurofeedback or cognitive training | Not receiving training | Individual: 40 sessions over 5 mo, 3 times/wk, 45 min/time | Conners 3–Parent Assessment Report, Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, and Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools |
van der Oord et al [ | ]Executive functioning training | Treatment as usual | Individual: 25 sessions over 5 wk, 40 min/session | Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning and Disruptive Behavior Disorder Rating Scale |
Wangler et al [ | ]Neurofeedback training | Computerized attention skills training | Individual: 3-4 wk, 25-30 min/session | Attention Network Test and ADHD Rating Scale |
aWISC-R: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised.
bWISC-IV: Backward Digit Span of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV.
cWMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale–III.
dADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Intervention Duration and Length
The intervention duration of 3 studies was 4 weeks. Meanwhile, the intervention duration of 9 studies was 5 to 8 weeks. One study had an intervention duration of 12 weeks, 2 studies had an intervention duration of 5 months, and 1 study had an intervention duration of 6 months. For the length of intervention, 1 trial was conducted for 15 minutes per session, 10 studies varied from 25 to 45 minutes per session, 2 studies varied from 45 to 60 minutes per session, and 1 study was conducted for 90 minutes per session.
Meta-Analysis Results of the Technology-Based Intervention
ADHD Behaviors
ADHD behaviors included in this study were inattention and hyperactivity or impulsivity. Moderate heterogeneity was observed among the studies examining ADHD behaviors (I2=26.4%).
shows the pooled results of ADHD behavior. Corkum et al [ ] evaluated the overall ADHD behaviors of participants, whereas the other 10 studies classified the results of the ADHD behaviors into inattention and hyperactivity or impulsivity. Corkum et al [ ] reported the Conners 3 Parent Rating Scale (Conners 3-P) and Conners 3 Teacher Rating Scale (Conners 3-T) scores. The SMD of ADHD behaviors measured with parent-rated evaluation was −0.21 (95% CI −0.73 to 0.31), and the teacher-rated evaluation of ADHD behaviors was −0.46 (95% CI −0.98 to 0.06). However, no significant effect was observed for ADHD behavior.A total of 12 studies reported inattention, which was measured using the ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS), Disruptive Behavior Disorder Rating Scale (DBDRS), Conners 3-P, and Conners 3-T. Parent-rated inattention measured by ADHD-RS (K=7), Conners 3-P (K=4), and DBDRS (K=2) had no statistically significant effect. Teacher-rated inattention measured by ADHD-RS (K=6), Conners 3-T (K=1), and DBDRS (K=2) had no significant effect. Computer-rated inattention measured by ADHD-RS (K=2; SMD −0.35, 95% CI −0.68 to −0.01) had a small and statistically significant effect (P<.04).
Ten studies explored the effectiveness of technology-based interventions on hyperactivity or impulsivity. Three different scales were used: ADHD-RS, DBDRS, and Conners 3-P and Conners 3-T. No significant effect was found in parent-rated hyperactivity or impulsivity measured by ADHD-RS (K=7), Conners 3-P (K=4), and DBDRS (K=2) and teacher-rated hyperactivity or impulsivity measured by ADHD-RS (K=4), Conners 3-T (K=1), and DBDRS (K=2).
The results of moderator analysis demonstrated that the sample size, setting, game elements, and type of control group moderated the effect size (
). A sample size of ≤50 (K=10; SMD −0.25, 95% CI −0.47 to −0.03; P<.03), nonhome setting (ie, clinic or school; K=12; SMD −0.24, 95% CI −0.37 to −0.11; P<.001), game elements excluded (K=40; SMD −0.15, 95% CI −0.23 to −0.07; P<.001), and control groups with no treatment (K=28; SMD −0.22, 95% CI −0.32 to −0.13; P<.001) and nonequivalent treatment had a significant moderating effect on the effect size.Variables | Number of effect sizes | Standardized mean difference | SE | 95% CI | z score | P value | |||||||
Parent-rated inattention | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RSa | 7 | −0.190 | 0.099 | −0.384 to 0.004 | −1.92 | .05 | |||||||
Conners 3-Pb | 4 | −0.373 | 0.144 | −0.655 to −0.092 | −2.60 | .10 | |||||||
DBDRSc | 2 | −0.462 | 0.212 | −0.878 to −0.045 | −2.17 | .09 | |||||||
Teacher-rated inattention | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RS | 6 | 0.052 | 0.077 | −0.098 to 0.202 | 0.678 | .50 | |||||||
Conners 3-Td | 1 | −0.150 | 0.318 | −0.774 to 0.474 | −0.472 | .64 | |||||||
DBDRS | 2 | −0.113 | 0.201 | −0.507 to 0.280 | −0.564 | .57 | |||||||
Computer-rated inattention | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RS | 2 | −0.345 | 0.17 | −0.679 to −0.011 | −2.025 | .043 | |||||||
Parent-rated hyperactivity or impulsivity | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RS | 7 | 0.032 | 0.14 | −0.243 to 0.306 | 0.225 | .82 | |||||||
Conners 3-P | 4 | −0.16 | 0.184 | −0.521 to 0.200 | −0.871 | .38 | |||||||
DBDRS | 2 | −0.311 | 0.267 | −0.834 to 0.212 | −1.164 | .24 | |||||||
Teacher-rated hyperactivity or impulsivity | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RS | 4 | 0.097 | 0.133 | −0.163 to 0.358 | 0.732 | .46 | |||||||
Conners 3-T | 1 | 0.312 | 0.318 | −0.312 to 0.935 | 0.979 | .33 | |||||||
DBDRS | 2 | −0.13 | 0.2 | −0.521 to 0.261 | −0.651 | .52 | |||||||
Parent-rated executive functions | |||||||||||||
BRIEFe | 2 | −0.347 | 0.17 | −0.681 to −0.013 | −2.038 | .04 | |||||||
Parent-rated inhibition | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 3 | −0.192 | 0.192 | −0.568 to 0.185 | −0.998 | .32 | |||||||
German Motor Test | 1 | −0.686 | 0.349 | −1.370 to −0.002 | −1.967 | .05 | |||||||
Computer-rated inhibition | |||||||||||||
BASCf | 1 | 0.231 | 0.274 | −0.306 to 0.768 | 0.844 | .40 | |||||||
BRIEF | 1 | 0.125 | 0.273 | −0.410 to 0.661 | 0.458 | .65 | |||||||
Parent-rated working memory | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 3 | −0.032 | 0.154 | −0.333 to 0.269 | −0.206 | .84 | |||||||
German Motor Test | 1 | 0.285 | 0.283 | −0.269 to 0.840 | 1.009 | .31 | |||||||
WISC-IVg | 1 | 0.788 | 0.266 | 0.267 to 1.309 | 2.965 | <.001 | |||||||
Computer-rated working memory | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.261 | 0.449 | −0.620 to 1.141 | 0.580 | .56 | |||||||
WISC-IV | 2 | 1.486 | 0.534 | 0.439 to 2.534 | 2.782 | .01 | |||||||
Parent-rated flexibility | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 3 | −0.142 | 0.259 | −0.649 to 0.365 | −0.549 | .58 | |||||||
German Motor Test | 1 | −0.566 | 0.457 | −1.462 to 0.331 | −1.237 | .22 | |||||||
Parent-rated emotional control | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.035 | 0.419 | −0.786 to 0.856 | 0.084 | .93 | |||||||
Parent-rated initiation | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.108 | 0.244 | −0.370 to 0.586 | 0.442 | .66 | |||||||
Teacher-rated initiation | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.196 | 0.169 | −0.136 to 0.528 | 1.156 | .25 | |||||||
Parent-rated planning and organization | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.057 | 0.253 | −0.438 to 0.553 | 0.227 | .82 | |||||||
Parent-rated organizing materials | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.118 | 0.177 | −0.230 to 0.465 | 0.663 | .51 | |||||||
Parent-rated monitoring | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | 0.291 | 0.327 | −0.349 to 0.932 | 0.891 | .37 | |||||||
Parent-rated metacognition | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 5 | −0.144 | 0.131 | −0.400 to 0.113 | −1.099 | .27 | |||||||
Teacher-rated metacognition | |||||||||||||
BRIEF | 2 | −0.116 | 0.171 | −0.451 to 0.220 | −0.676 | .50 | |||||||
Parent-rated disruptive behavior disorder | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RS | 1 | 0.350 | 0.242 | −0.124 to 0.825 | 1.447 | .15 | |||||||
BOSSh | 2 | −0.187 | 0.170 | −0.521 to 0.147 | −1.096 | .27 | |||||||
CBCi | 3 | −0.504 | 0.111 | −0.720 to −0.287 | −4.550 | <.001 | |||||||
DBDRS | 5 | −0.306 | 0.125 | −0.552 to −0.060 | −2.441 | .02 | |||||||
FBB-SSVj | 3 | −0.127 | 0.154 | −0.428 to 0.174 | −0.826 | .41 | |||||||
Teacher-rated disruptive behavior disorder | |||||||||||||
ADHD-RS | 1 | 0.076 | 0.239 | −0.392 to 0.545 | 0.319 | .75 | |||||||
DBDRS | 4 | 0.009 | 0.141 | −0.267 to 0.286 | 0.065 | .95 | |||||||
Computer-rated visual attention | |||||||||||||
ANTk | 5 | 0.014 | 0.112 | −0.207 to 0.234 | 0.121 | .90 | |||||||
ASTl | 2 | −0.114 | 0.183 | −0.473 to 0.244 | −0.624 | .53 | |||||||
BVRTm | 1 | 0.000 | 0.263 | −0.516 to 0.516 | 0.000 | >.99 | |||||||
CPTn | 8 | −0.419 | 0.124 | −0.662 to −0.176 | −3.377 | <.001 | |||||||
IEDo | 1 | −0.313 | 0.260 | −0.822 to 0.196 | −1.205 | .23 | |||||||
IVA/CPTp | 6 | −0.250 | 0.132 | −0.508 to 0.009 | −1.892 | .06 | |||||||
RTIq | 2 | −0.425 | 0.186 | −0.790 to −0.060 | −2.280 | .02 | |||||||
RVIPr | 4 | −0.021 | 0.130 | −0.275 to 0.233 | −0.165 | .87 | |||||||
Computer-rated auditory attention | |||||||||||||
CAVLT-2s | 5 | 0.011 | 0.147 | −0.276 to 0.298 | 0.074 | .94 | |||||||
IVA/CPT | 4 | −0.084 | 0.195 | −0.467 to 0.298 | −0.431 | .67 | |||||||
Parent-rated sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward | |||||||||||||
SPSRQ-Ct | 4 | −0.147 | 0.128 | −0.399 to 0.104 | −1.148 | .25 | |||||||
Parent-rated quality of life | |||||||||||||
PedsQLu | 1 | 0.646 | 0.263 | 0.131 to 1.161 | 2.458 | .01 | |||||||
Self-rated quality of life | |||||||||||||
PedsQL | 1 | 0.042 | 0.256 | −0.460 to 0.544 | 0.163 | .87 | |||||||
Computer-rated reading fluency | |||||||||||||
LOGOSv | 2 | 0.014 | 0.186 | −0.351 to 0.379 | 0.073 | .94 | |||||||
Parent-rated adherence to stimulants | |||||||||||||
Adherence to stimulants | 1 | −0.113 | 0.125 | −0.358 to 0.132 | −0.906 | .37 |
aADHD-RS: ADHD Rating Scale.
bConners 3-P: Conners 3 Parent Rating Scale.
cDBDRS: Disruptive Behavior Disorder Rating Scale.
dConners 3-T: Conners 3 Teacher Rating Scale.
eBRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function.
fBASC: Behavior Assessment System for Children.
gWISC-IV: Backward Digit Span of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV.
hBOSS: Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools.
iCBC: Child Behavior Checklist.
jFBB-SSV: Fremdbeurteilungsbogen für Störungen des Sozialverhaltens.
kANT: Attention Network Test.
lAST: Attention Switching Task.
mBVRT: Benton Visual Retention Test.
nCPT: Continuous Performance Test.
oIED: Intraextra Dimensional Set Shift.
pIVA/CPT: Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test.
qRTI: Reaction Time.
rRVIP: Rapid Visual Information Processing.
sCAVLT-2: Children’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test-2.
tSPSRQ-C: Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire for Children.
uPedsQL: Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory.
vLOGOS: Logometrica.
Number of effect sizes | Standardized mean difference | SE | 95% CI | z score | P value | Heterogeneity | ||||
Q-statistics | I2 | |||||||||
ADHD behavior | 46 | −0.105 | 0.037 | −0.177 to −0.033 | −2.853 | <.001 | 61.123 | 26.378 | ||
Number of sessions | N/Aa | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.312 | .02 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤20 | 11 | −0.012 | 0.094 | −0.195 to 0.172 | −0.124 | .90 | ||||
>20 to 40 | 26 | −0.111 | 0.060 | −0.229 to 0.007 | −1.841 | .07 | ||||
>40 to 60 | 4 | −0.163 | 0.143 | −0.443 to 0.117 | −1.140 | .25 | ||||
>60 | 2 | −0.254 | 0.153 | −0.554 to 0.045 | −1.663 | .10 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.651 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤50 | 10 | −0.245 | 0.112 | −0.465 to −0.025 | −2.183 | .03 | ||||
>50 to 100 | 29 | −0.076 | 0.057 | −0.188 to 0.037 | −1.320 | .19 | ||||
>100 | 7 | −0.143 | 0.092 | −0.324 to 0.037 | −1.555 | .12 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.651 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
Home | 21 | −0.102 | 0.056 | −0.212 to 0.007 | −1.827 | .07 | ||||
Mixed | 8 | 0.204 | 0.107 | −0.006 to 0.414 | 1.905 | .06 | ||||
Nonhome | 12 | −0.242 | 0.066 | −0.372 to −0.113 | −3.670 | <.001 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.800 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 40 | −0.151 | 0.043 | −0.234 to −0.068 | −3.546 | <.001 | ||||
Game elements included | 1 | 0.241 | 0.131 | −0.015 to 0.497 | 1.845 | .07 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.756 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
Equivalent treatment | 9 | 0.083 | 0.071 | −0.057 to 0.223 | 1.161 | .25 | ||||
No treatment | 28 | −0.225 | 0.047 | −0.317 to −0.132 | −4.749 | <.001 | ||||
Nonequivalent treatment | 4 | 0.337 | 0.165 | 0.013 to 0.661 | 2.041 | .04 | ||||
Executive functions | 48 | 0.013 | 0.037 | −0.060 to 0.087 | 0.357 | .72 | 99.361 | 52.698 | ||
Number of sessions | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.257 | .80 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤20 | 3 | 0.777 | 0.303 | 0.183 to 1.371 | 2.564 | .01 | ||||
>20 to 40 | 43 | −0.012 | 0.056 | −0.121 to 0.097 | −0.214 | .83 | ||||
>40 to 60 | 2 | 0.012 | 0.253 | −0.484 to 0.508 | 0.048 | .96 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.260 | .80 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤50 | 7 | −0.046 | 0.168 | −0.376 to 0.284 | −0.272 | .79 | ||||
>50 to 100 | 37 | 0.063 | 0.058 | −0.051 to 0.177 | 1.082 | .28 | ||||
>100 | 4 | −0.363 | 0.174 | −0.704 to −0.023 | −2.093 | .04 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.257 | .80 | N/A | N/A | ||
Home | 40 | 0.047 | 0.059 | −0.068 to 0.163 | 0.802 | .42 | ||||
Mixed | 2 | 0.178 | 0.268 | −0.347 to 0.703 | 0.664 | .51 | ||||
Nonhome | 6 | −0.239 | 0.146 | −0.525 to 0.047 | −1.641 | .10 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.260 | .80 | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 42 | 0.009 | 0.055 | −0.099 to 0.116 | 0.161 | .87 | ||||
Game elements included | 3 | −0.318 | 0.219 | −0.748 to 0.112 | −1.448 | .15 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.257 | .80 | N/A | N/A | ||
Equivalent treatment | 11 | −0.022 | 0.111 | −0.240 to 0.196 | −0.196 | .84 | ||||
No treatment | 34 | −0.007 | 0.062 | −0.129 to 0.115 | −0.118 | .91 | ||||
Nonequivalent treatment | 3 | 0.777 | 0.303 | 0.183 to 1.370 | 2.565 | .01 | ||||
Disruptive behavior disorder | 19 | −0.212 | 0.057 | −0.323 to −0.101 | −3.735 | <.001 | 27.160 | 33.725 | ||
Number of sessions | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −3.307 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤20 | 1 | −1.070 | 0.475 | −2.001 to −0.138 | −2.251 | .02 | ||||
>20 to 40 | 13 | −0.102 | 0.078 | −0.254 to 0.050 | −1.312 | .19 | ||||
>60 | 2 | −0.470 | 0.128 | −0.722 to −0.219 | −3.668 | <.001 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −3.422 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤50 | 5 | −0.395 | 0.156 | −0.702 to −0.089 | −2.527 | .01 | ||||
>50 to 100 | 10 | −0.035 | 0.084 | −0.200 to 0.131 | −0.411 | .68 | ||||
>100 | 4 | −0.377 | 0.102 | −0.577 to −0.176 | −3.675 | <.001 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.714 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
Home | 12 | −0.127 | 0.096 | −0.314 to 0.061 | −1.322 | .19 | ||||
Nonhome | 4 | −0.361 | 0.130 | −0.017 to −0.616 | −2.781 | .01 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.738 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 19 | −0.196 | 0.072 | −0.337 to −0.056 | −2.738 | .01 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.911 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
Equivalent treatment | 8 | −0.106 | 0.106 | −0.314 to 0.102 | −1.001 | .32 | ||||
No treatment | 10 | −0.235 | 0.090 | −0.412 to −0.058 | −2.606 | .01 | ||||
Nonequivalent treatment | 1 | −1.070 | 0.491 | −2.032 to −0.108 | −2.179 | .03 | ||||
Visual attention | 29 | −0.174 | 0.050 | −0.273 to −0.076 | −3.487 | <.001 | 36.296 | 22.856 | ||
Number of sessions | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −3.415 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤20 | 5 | −0.355 | 0.185 | −0.718 to 0.009 | −1.914 | .06 | ||||
>20 to 40 | 18 | −0.232 | 0.077 | −0.383 to −0.080 | −3.000 | <.001 | ||||
>40 to 60 | 1 | 0.000 | 0.307 | −0.602 to 0.602 | 0.000 | >.99 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −3.112 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
≤50 | 2 | −0.025 | 0.315 | −0.642 to 0.592 | −0.080 | .94 | ||||
>50 to 100 | 27 | −0.187 | 0.059 | −0.303 to −0.071 | −3.152 | <.001 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −2.810 | .01 | N/A | N/A | ||
Home | 12 | −0.162 | 0.095 | −0.349 to 0.025 | −1.700 | .10 | ||||
Mixed | 8 | −0.323 | 0.127 | −0.571 to −0.075 | −2.550 | .01 | ||||
Nonhome | 1 | 0.000 | 0.312 | −0.612 to 0.612 | 0.000 | >.99 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −3.146 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 27 | −0.187 | 0.059 | −0.303 to −0.071 | −3.152 | <.001 | ||||
Game elements included | 2 | −0.025 | 0.315 | −0.642 to 0.592 | −0.080 | .94 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −3.146 | <.001 | N/A | N/A | ||
Equivalent treatment | 13 | −0.267 | 0.086 | −0.435 to −0.099 | −3.109 | <.001 | ||||
No treatment | 10 | −0.147 | 0.089 | −0.322 to 0.028 | −1.647 | .10 | ||||
Nonequivalent treatment | 6 | −0.003 | 0.155 | −0.306 to 0.300 | −0.017 | .99 | —b | — | ||
Auditory attention | 9 | −0.020 | 0.091 | −0.199 to 0.159 | −0.218 | .83 | 11.641 | 31.279 | ||
Number of sessions | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −0.200 | .84 | N/A | N/A | ||
>20 to 40 | 4 | −0.084 | 0.195 | −0.467 to 0.298 | −0.431 | .67 | ||||
>40 to 60 | 5 | 0.011 | 0.147 | −0.276 to 0.298 | 0.074 | .94 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −0.205 | .84 | N/A | N/A | ||
>50 to 100 | 9 | −0.023 | 0.111 | −0.240 to 0.195 | −0.205 | .84 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −0.200 | .84 | N/A | N/A | ||
Mixed | 4 | −0.084 | 0.195 | −0.467 to 0.298 | −0.431 | .67 | ||||
Nonhome | 5 | 0.011 | 0.147 | −0.276 to 0.298 | 0.074 | .94 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −0.205 | .84 | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 9 | −0.023 | 0.111 | −0.240 to 0.195 | −0.205 | .84 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | −0.200 | .84 | N/A | N/A | ||
No treatment | 5 | 0.011 | 0.147 | −0.276 to 0.298 | 0.074 | .94 | ||||
Nonequivalent treatment | 4 | −0.084 | 0.195 | −0.467 to 0.298 | −0.431 | .67 | ||||
Sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward | 4 | −0.147 | 0.128 | −0.399 to 0.104 | −1.148 | >.99 | 0.000 | 0.000 | ||
Quality of life | 2 | 0.336 | 0.183 | −0.023 to 0.696 | 1.833 | .07 | 2.712 | 63.124 | ||
Number of sessions | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 1.129 | .26 | N/A | N/A | ||
>20 to 40 | 2 | 0.341 | 0.302 | −0.251 to 0.933 | 1.129 | .26 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | ||
>50 to 100 | 2 | 0.341 | 0.302 | −0.251 to 0.933 | 1.129 | .26 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | ||
Home | 2 | 0.341 | 0.302 | −0.251 to 0.933 | 1.129 | .26 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 2 | 0.341 | 0.302 | −0.251 to 0.933 | 1.129 | .26 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | ||
Equivalent treatment | 2 | 0.341 | 0.302 | −0.251 to 0.933 | 1.129 | .26 | ||||
Reading fluency | 2 | 0.014 | 0.173 | −0.326 to 0.353 | 0.079 | .94 | 1.156 | 13.528 | ||
Number of sessions | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.073 | .94 | N/A | N/A | ||
>40 to 60 | 2 | 0.014 | 0.186 | −0.351 to 0.379 | 0.073 | .94 | ||||
Sample size | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.073 | .94 | N/A | N/A | ||
>50 to 100 | 2 | 0.014 | 0.186 | −0.351 to 0.379 | 0.073 | .94 | ||||
Setting | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.073 | .94 | N/A | N/A | ||
Nonhome | 2 | 0.014 | 0.186 | −0.351 to 0.379 | 0.073 | .94 | ||||
Game elements | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.073 | .94 | N/A | N/A | ||
Game elements excluded | 2 | 0.014 | 0.186 | −0.351 to 0.379 | 0.073 | .94 | ||||
Types of control group | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | 0.073 | .94 | N/A | N/A | ||
No treatment | 2 | 0.014 | 0.186 | −0.351 to 0.379 | 0.073 | .94 |
aN/A: not applicable.
bNot available.
Executive Functions
The fundamental skills of executive functions include inhibition, working memory, flexibility, emotional control, initiation, planning and organization, organizing materials, monitoring, and metacognition. Substantial heterogeneity was observed among the studies that examined executive functions (I2=52.7%).
shows the pooled results of the fundamental skills of executive functions. Steiner et al [ ] reported an overall executive function of school-aged children with ADHD and SMD using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) score. The SMD was −0.35 (95% CI −0.68 to −0.01), indicating a small and statistically significant effect was observed for the overall executive function.Five studies reported inhibition through BRIEF, Behavior Assessment System for Children, and German Motor Test. Parent-rated inhibition using BRIEF (K=3) and German Motor Test (K=1) showed no significant effect. No significant effect was observed for computer-rated inhibition measured by BRIEF (K=1) and Behavior Assessment System for Children (K=1).
Six studies investigated working memory. Three different scales were used: the Backward Digit Span of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV (WISC-IV), BRIEF, and German Motor Test. No significant effect was observed for parent-rated working memory measured by BRIEF (K=3), German Motor Test (K=1), and WISC-IV (K=1). A large and statistically significant effect favoring the control group was observed for computer-rated working memory measured by WISC-IV (K=2; SMD 1.49, 95% CI 0.44-2.53).
Four studies evaluated flexibility using BRIEF and German Motor Test. No significant result was observed for parent-rated flexibility measured by BRIEF (K=3) and German Motor Test (K=1).
Two studies reported emotional control, and the BRIEF score was used for the measurement. No significant effect was observed for parent-rated emotional control and teacher-rated emotional control.
Initiation was reported by 2 studies using BRIEF to score. No statistically significant result was found for parent-rated initiation and teacher-rated initiation.
Three studies used the BRIEF assessment to score and report the impact of technology-based interventions on planning and organization. No significant effect was found for planning and organization for parent-rated planning and organization.
Two studies reported the BRIEF scores for organizing materials. No significant result was observed for parent-rated organizing materials.
Two studies reported the BRIEF scores for monitoring. The results of parent-rated monitoring were not statistically significant.
Four studies explored the effects of technology-based interventions on metacognition using BRIEF. The results for parent-rated and teacher-rated metacognition were not statistically significant.
The results of moderator analysis showed no moderating effects among the moderators (
).Disruptive Behavior Disorder
Conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder are the most common disruptive behavior disorders. Seven studies reported the effectiveness of technology-based interventions on disruptive behavior disorder. Moderate heterogeneity was observed among these studies (I2=33.7%).
shows the pooled results of disruptive behavior disorder. Five different scales were adopted: ADHD-RS, Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools, Child Behavior Checklist, DBDRS, and German Rating Scale for Oppositional Defiant and Conduct Disorders. Small and significant effects were observed for parent-rated disruptive behavior disorder measured by Child Behavior Checklist (K=3; SMD −0.50, 95% CI −0.72 to −0.29) and DBDRS (K=5; SMD −0.31, 95% CI −0.55 to −0.06). Parent-rated disruptive behavior disorder measured by ADHD-RS (K=1), Behavioral Observation of Students in Schools (K=2), and German Rating Scale for Oppositional Defiant and Conduct Disorders (K=3) had no significant effect. No significant effect was observed for teacher-rated disruptive behavior disorder measured using ADHD-RS and DBDRS.The results of the moderator analysis demonstrated that the number of sessions, sample size, setting, game elements, and type of control group moderated the effect size (
). The number of sessions of >60 (K=2; SMD −0.47, 95% CI −0.72 to −0.22; P<.001), sample size of ≤50 (K=5; SMD −0.40, 95% CI −0.70 to −0.09; P<.01), and >100 (K=4; SMD −0.38, 95% CI −0.58 to −0.18; P<.001); nonhome setting (K=4; SMD −0.36, 95% CI −0.02 to −0.62; P<.005); game elements excluded (K=19; SMD −0.20, 95% CI −0.34 to −0.06; P<.006); and no treatment (K=10; SMD −0.24, 95% CI −0.41 to −0.06; P<.009) moderated the effect size.Visual Attention
Eight studies explored the effects of technology-based visual attention. There was a small heterogeneity among these studies (I2=22.9%).
shows the pooled results of visual attention. Four studies used the Continuous Performance Test as measurement, whereas the other study used the Benton Visual Retention Test, Phonological and Visuospatial Reordering, and Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA/CPT). In addition, 1 study used the Attention Network Test, Attention Switching Task, Rapid Visual Information Processing, Intraextra Dimensional Set Shift, and Reaction Time to score visual attention. Technology-based intervention had small and significant effects on visual attention measured by Continuous Performance Test (K=8; SMD −0.42, 95% CI −0.66 to −0.18) and Reaction Time (K=2; SMD −0.43, 95% CI −0.79 to −0.06). No significant effect was observed for visual attention measured using Attention Network Test, Attention Network Test, Benton Visual Retention Test, Intraextra Dimensional Set Shift, IVA/CPT, and Rapid Visual Information Processing.The results of the moderator analysis showed that the number of sessions, sample size, setting, game elements, and type of control group moderated the effect size (
). The number of sessions of >20 to 40 (K=18; SMD −0.23, 95% CI −0.38 to −0.08; P<.003), sample size of >than 50 to 100 (K=27; SMD −0.19, 95% CI −0.30 to −0.07; P<.002), mixed setting (ie, combining both home and school or clinic; K=8; SMD −0.32, 95% CI −0.57 to −0.08; P<.01), game elements excluded (K=27; SMD −0.19, 95% CI −0.30 to −0.07; P<.002), and equivalent treatment (K=13; SMD −0.27, 95% CI −0.44 to −0.10; P<.002) moderated the effect size.Auditory Attention
Two studies evaluated auditory attention, which was scored using 2 scales, namely, Children’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test-2 (CAVLT-2) and IVA/CPT. No statistically significant effect was observed for auditory attention measured using these 2 scales (
). Moderate heterogeneity was observed among these studies (I2=31.3%).The results of moderator analysis showed no moderating effects among the moderators (
).Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward
The study by Dovis et al [
] reported the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire for Children score. A nonsignificant effect was found for parent-rated sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward ( ). Homogeneity was observed for these records ( ).Quality of Life
Dovis et al [
] reported the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (PedsQL) score. No significant effect was observed for parent-rated quality of life and self-rated quality of life ( ). Substantial heterogeneity was observed among these records (I2=63.1%).The results of moderator analysis showed no moderating effects among the moderators (
).Reading Fluency
Egeland et al [
] investigated the effectiveness of a technology-based intervention on reading fluency. Logometrica was used to score reading fluency. The result of reading fluency was not at a statistically significant level ( ). Small heterogeneity was observed among these records (I2=13.5%).The results of moderator analysis showed no moderating effects among the moderators (
).Adherence to Stimulants
Fried et al [
] evaluated adherence to stimulants. There was no significant effect on adherence to stimulants ( ). Heterogeneity estimation was not conducted because of 1 effect size in adherence to stimulants. No moderator analysis was conducted because of only 1 effect size for adherence to stimulants ( ).Discussion
Principal Findings
The adoption of technologies in school-age children with ADHD has become prevalent in this decade and has been applied in many countries. Nevertheless, the efficacy of applying technology-based treatments for children has not been well established. To the best of our knowledge, this work is the first systematic review and meta-analysis of RCTs to assess the efficacy of technology-based interventions for school-age children, which provides strong evidence for health care practitioners. The pooled results indicated that technology-based treatment has the potential to regulate the inattention, overall executive function, and visual attention of school-age children with ADHD.
Quality of Evidence
Most of the included studies were of moderate quality, and only 2 studies [
, ] fulfilled the 7 criteria for randomized trials. More than half of the included studies had an unblinded or unclear method of randomization and allocation concealment. Although the blinding of participants was unpreventably broken owing to the nature of the intervention in most studies, the level of influence on the outcomes was considered low in most studies. Only 6 studies were assessed by blinded assessors. The attrition rate in all interventions did not exceed 20%, which met the criteria of the dropout rate. Only 8 studies reported the intention-to-treat analysis. Thus, more rigorous methods should be designed to scientifically justify the effects of technology-based interventions for children with ADHD.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on ADHD Behaviors
The pooled results indicated that technology-based treatment may have the potential to improve computer-rated inattention. No significant results were found for parent-rated and teacher-rated inattention and parent-rated and teacher-rated hyperactivity or impulsivity. This finding is consistent with that of previous studies, which showed that computer-assisted training had a positive effect on inattention [
, ]. Moreover, neurofeedback training and brain-computer interfaces training involve feeding brain activity into a computer, which maps the areas of the brain [ ]. The patient controls a computer or video game by producing a brief, sustained brainwave activity in the target area, thereby training the brain regions that are aroused. These treatments include cognitive training to remediate the inattention behaviors of patients with ADHD. The insignificant finding on hyperactivity or impulsivity may be explained by the harmful effect of screen time and the frequent digital media use, which seem to increase the risk of the symptoms of hyperactivity or impulsivity. A meta-analysis conducted by Nikkelen et al [ ] identified a modest association between the use of traditional digital media and ADHD behaviors. Notifications and invitation messages may pop up in these training programs. Exposure to these notifications and messages may take away the attention of a child from the task, possibly disrupting the normative development of patience and impulse control [ ]. The precise descriptions of computer training, neurofeedback training, and virtual reality have not been completely demonstrated. Thus, relevant information is needed to provide further studies to assess the impact of the messages that suddenly pop up regarding the symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity among children with ADHD. Furthermore, it was found that a smaller sample size, nonhome setting, game elements excluded, and no treatment and nonequivalent treatment as the control group tended to have more improving effects on ADHD behavior while conducting the technology-based intervention. This suggests that fewer participants, conducting the intervention at clinic and school, intervention without game elements, and using no treatment or nontechnological intervention as control groups may increase the potential of the technology-based intervention to alleviate ADHD behaviors.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Executive Functions
Nine categories, namely, inhibition, working memory, flexibility, emotional control, initiation, planning and organization, organizing materials, monitoring, and metacognition, were used to evaluate the executive functions. The pooled result showed a statistically significant effect of the technology-based intervention on parent-rated executive functions. A large significant effect of the control group on computer-rated working memory compared with the technological intervention was found, indicating computed-rated working memory would favor the control group instead of technological intervention. No significant effects were found from other categories related to executive functions. These findings were unexpected, as most previous studies reported positive effects of technology-based training on the improvement of executive functions [
, , ].Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Disruptive Behavior Disorder
Evidence indicated that technology-based interventions and computer-assisted training may regulate both parent-rated disruptive behavior disorders. This finding is consistent with the previous case study by Kotwal et al [
], who found that the frequency of parent-reported disruptive behaviors in a boy with ADHD was reduced at home and school after conducting a 3-month computer-assisted training. More number of sessions, small and large sample sizes, nonhome settings, game elements excluded, and no treatment as the control group seemed to improve the disruptive behavior disorder when adopting the technology-based intervention. It is unexpected that small and large sample sizes might moderate the association between technology-based interventions and disruptive behavior disorder. Thus, further investigations need to be conducted to evaluate the moderating effect of different sample sizes.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Visual Attention
Participants’ ability to take in important visual information seems to be significantly improved after conducting computer-assisted training. Ordikhani-Seyedlar et al [
] used brain-computer interfaces with electroencephalograms to generate neurofeedback that significantly improved the visual attention of patients with ADHD. In addition, numerous studies have reported the positive outcomes of adopting neurofeedback training in patients with ADHD [ , ]. Alpha-beta activities in the electroencephalogram have been used to evaluate the attention level of the participants [ ]. Furthermore, moderate sessions, moderate sample size, mixed setting, and game elements excluded and using equivalent treatment as a control might improve visual attention under the technology-based interventions. It is somewhat surprising that a mixed setting and equivalent treatment as the control group may have a moderating effect; thus, more RCTs should be conducted to identify the underlying reasons.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Auditory Attention
The pooled result showed a nonsignificant result on auditory attention. However, some experimental studies have reported the significant effect of biofeedback technology on strengthening auditory attention in children with ADHD [
]. Auditory attention among children with ADHD may be influenced by the intensity and frequency of sound [ , ]. White noise therapy has the potential to improve speech recognition and auditory attention in children with ADHD [ , ]. White noise, which is a random signal with equal intensity at different frequencies, is a steady, unchanging, and unobtrusive sound from certain machines such as a whirring fan and a static radio. The intensity and frequency of sound can remarkably influence the auditory attention of children with ADHD.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Reading Fluency
No statistically significant effect was found for reading fluency. Nevertheless, previous experiments reported an improvement in oral reading fluency through a computerized program, Headsprout [
]. The computerized program could enhance the engagement of students with ADHD. Hence, learning among these students becomes more effective. This observation also implies that technology, such as a computerized program, has a high potential to enhance oral reading fluency. More RCTs with a larger pediatric population are needed to evaluate the effects of technology on reading fluency among children with ADHD.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Adherence to Stimulants
Our result regarding the effects of the technology-based intervention on adherence to stimulants was not statistically significant. The literature review conducted by Chacko et al [
] indicated that technology-based interventions could reduce environmental barriers, such as caregivers’ forgetfulness, to improve medication adherence for the pediatric population with ADHD. This nonsignificant finding may be explained by the fact that only a small number of RCTs were conducted to determine the effects of using technologies on stimulant adherence in children with ADHD.Effects of Technology-Based Intervention on Sensitivity to Punishment, Sensitivity to Reward, and Quality of Life
A narrative review and pooled results found that technology had no significant effects on sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward and quality of life. Few qualitative and quantitative studies have been conducted to investigate the effectiveness of technology-based interventions on sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward and quality of life among children with ADHD. This circumstance indicates that more trials should be conducted.
Limitations and Future Research
This study is the first systematic review and meta-analysis of RCTs to examine the effectiveness of technology-based treatment for school-age children with ADHD. The strengths of this review include the use of well-defined inclusive and exclusive criteria, the application of a rigorous and well-constructed search strategy from the 7 electronic databases, and the stringent quality assessment of selected RCTs. However, this study has some limitations. First, the small sample size of the included trials implies the sparsity of RCTs in the children-computer interaction research field. Second, the pooled results may be influenced by variations in intervention designs (ie, intervention intervals and duration) because of the considerable clinical heterogeneity. Third, more than half of the included RCTs were designed using a nonblind approach owing to the unavoidable physical component of the interventions. To minimize these biases, blinded outcome assessors are strongly recommended for open-label RCTs. Fourth, the age selection was between 6 and 12 years, which may exclude preschool age children and adolescents with ADHD benefiting from technology-based treatment. Finally, RCTs published in languages other than English were excluded. Thus, language bias may be premeditated. Although RCT provide excellent internal validity and the most reliable evidence, the results are needed to be interpreted with caution owing to the inclusion of the designated population in this review. Furthermore, the use of virtual reality–based treatments for children with ADHD has been gradually implemented in the field of developmental psychology. RCTs assessing the effects of virtual reality–based treatments on children are still limited; thus, more relevant research is recommended. Moreover, the safety issues of children’s interactions with technological interventions have rarely been discussed in RCTs. Measurements of side effects can be adopted in studies to further optimize the designs of the technological treatment, which can facilitate the universality of technological therapies in patients of different ages.
Conclusions
This review synthesized evidence from 19 RCTs on the application of technology-based interventions in school-age children with ADHD. The results indicated that the existing RCTs were mainly of low to moderate quality, particularly in random sequence generation, allocation of concealment and blinding, and intention-to-treat analysis. Further well-designed and rigorous trials should be conducted to determine the effectiveness of technology-based interventions in children with ADHD. The pooled results indicated that children’s intervention with technological treatments has effects on computer-rated inattention, parent-rated overall executive functions, parent-rated disruptive behavior disorder, and visual attention. The number of sample sizes, setting of the intervention, exclusion of game elements, and type of control group had moderating effects on ADHD behaviors, disruptive behavior disorder, and visual attention. The potential of technology-based therapies for hyperactivity or impulsivity, inhibition, working memory, flexibility, emotional control, initiation, planning and organization, organizing materials, monitoring, metacognition, disruptive behavior disorder, visual attention, auditory attention, sensitivity to punishment and reward, quality of life, adherence to stimulants, and reading fluency requires further evaluation.
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
Full search strategy.
DOCX File , 15 KBPRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) 2020 checklist.
PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 81 KBReferences
- Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics. Jul 2012;9(3):490-499. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Salari N, Ghasemi H, Abdoli N, Rahmani A, Shiri MH, Hashemian AH, et al. The global prevalence of ADHD in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ital J Pediatr. Apr 20, 2023;49(1):48. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edition. New York City, NY. American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
- Colomer C, Berenguer C, Roselló B, Baixauli I, Miranda A. The impact of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms, and executive functions on learning behaviors of children with ADHD. Front Psychol. Apr 12, 2017;8:540. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Van der Oord S, Van der Meulen EM, Prins PJ, Oosterlaan J, Buitelaar JK, Emmelkamp PM. A psychometric evaluation of the social skills rating system in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Res Ther. Jun 2005;43(6):733-746. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Biederman J, Faraone S, Milberger S, Curtis S, Chen L, Marrs A, et al. Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: results from a four-year prospective follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Mar 1996;35(3):343-351. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Barbaresi WJ, Colligan RC, Weaver AL, Voigt RG, Killian JM, Katusic SK. Mortality, ADHD, and psychosocial adversity in adults with childhood ADHD: a prospective study. Pediatrics. Apr 2013;131(4):637-644. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Briars L, Todd T. A review of pharmacological management of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Pediatr Pharmacol Ther. May 2016;21(3):192-206. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lim CG, Ooi YP, Fung DS, Mahendran R, Kaur A. Sleep disturbances in Singaporean children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Ann Acad Med Singap. Aug 2008;37(8):655-661. [FREE Full text] [Medline]
- Goldfried MR, Castonguay LG. Behavior therapy: redefining strengths and limitations. Behav Ther. 1993;24(4):505-526. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Farzanfar R, Frishkopf S, Friedman R, Ludena K. Evaluating an automated mental health care system: making meaning of human–computer interaction. Comput Human Behav. May 2007;23(3):1167-1182. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Olson GM, Olson JS. Human-computer interaction: psychological aspects of the human use of computing. Annu Rev Psychol. Feb 2003;54(1):491-516. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Alneyadi M, Drissi N, Almeqbaali M, Ouhbi S. Biofeedback-based connected mental health interventions for anxiety: systematic literature review. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. Apr 22, 2021;9(4):e26038. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schueller SM, Stiles-Shields C, Yarosh L. Online treatment and virtual therapists in child and adolescent psychiatry. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. Jan 2017;26(1):1-12. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Baum RA, Epstein JN, Kelleher K. Healthcare reform, quality, and technology: ADHD as a case study. Curr Psychiatry Rep. Jul 2013;15(7):369. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rapport MD, Chung KM, Shore G, Denney CB, Isaacs P. Upgrading the science and technology of assessment and diagnosis: laboratory and clinic-based assessment of children with ADHD. J Clin Child Psychol. Dec 2000;29(4):555-568. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Nikkelen SW, Valkenburg PM, Huizinga M, Bushman BJ. Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Dev Psychol. Sep 2014;50(9):2228-2241. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Scionti N, Cavallero M, Zogmaister C, Marzocchi GM. Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. Jan 10, 2019;10:2812. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Dovis S, Van der Oord S, Wiers RW, Prins PJ. Improving executive functioning in children with ADHD: training multiple executive functions within the context of a computer game. a randomized double-blind placebo controlled trial. PLoS One. Apr 06, 2015;10(4):e0121651. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Moreno-García I, Meneres-Sancho S, Camacho-Vara de Rey C, Servera M. A randomized controlled trial to examine the posttreatment efficacy of neurofeedback, behavior therapy, and pharmacology on ADHD measures. J Atten Disord. Feb 01, 2019;23(4):374-383. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Egeland J, Aarlien AK, Saunes BK. Few effects of far transfer of working memory training in ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. Oct 4, 2013;8(10):e75660. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Steiner NJ, Frenette EC, Rene KM, Brennan RT, Perrin EC. In-school neurofeedback training for ADHD: sustained improvements from a randomized control trial. Pediatrics. Mar 2014;133(3):483-492. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- van der Oord S, Ponsioen AJ, Geurts HM, Ten Brink EL, Prins PJ. A pilot study of the efficacy of a computerized executive functioning remediation training with game elements for children with ADHD in an outpatient setting: outcome on parent- and teacher-rated executive functioning and ADHD behavior. J Atten Disord. Nov 2014;18(8):699-712. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bikic A, Leckman JF, Christensen T, Bilenberg N, Dalsgaard S. Attention and executive functions computer training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): results from a randomized, controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Dec 2018;27(12):1563-1574. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kofler MJ, Wells EL, Singh LJ, Soto EF, Irwin LN, Groves NB, et al. A randomized controlled trial of central executive training (CET) versus inhibitory control training (ICT) for ADHD. J Consult Clin Psychol. Aug 2020;88(8):738-756. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Meyer KN, Santillana R, Miller B, Clapp W, Way M, Bridgman-Goines K, et al. Computer-based inhibitory control training in children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): evidence for behavioral and neural impact. PLoS One. Nov 30, 2020;15(11):e0241352. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Benzing V, Schmidt M. The effect of exergaming on executive functions in children with ADHD: a randomized clinical trial. Scand J Med Sci Sports. Aug 2019;29(8):1243-1253. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Azami S, Moghadas A, Sohrabi-Esmrood F, Nazifi M, Mirmohamad M, Hemmati F, et al. A pilot randomized controlled trial comparing computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation, stimulant medication, and an active control in the treatment of ADHD. Child Adolesc Ment Health. Nov 2016;21(4):217-224. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lim CG, Poh XW, Fung SS, Guan C, Bautista D, Cheung YB, et al. A randomized controlled trial of a brain-computer interface based attention training program for ADHD. PLoS One. May 21, 2019;14(5):e0216225. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Breider S, de Bildt A, Nauta MH, Hoekstra PJ, van den Hoofdakker BJ. Self-directed or therapist-led parent training for children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? A randomized controlled non-inferiority pilot trial. Internet Interv. Aug 08, 2019;18:100262. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wangler S, Gevensleben H, Albrecht B, Studer P, Rothenberger A, Moll GH, et al. Neurofeedback in children with ADHD: specific event-related potential findings of a randomized controlled trial. Clin Neurophysiol. May 2011;122(5):942-950. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Bigorra A, Garolera M, Guijarro S, Hervas A. Impact of working memory training on hot executive functions (decision-making and theory of mind) in children with ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychiatry. 2016;6(5) [CrossRef]
- Bioulac S, Micoulaud-Franchi JA, Maire J, Bouvard MP, Rizzo AA, Sagaspe P, et al. Virtual remediation versus methylphenidate to improve distractibility in children with ADHD: a controlled randomized clinical trial study. J Atten Disord. Jan 2020;24(2):326-335. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Cibrian FL, Lakes KD, Schuck SE, Hayes GR. The potential for emerging technologies to support self-regulation in children with ADHD: a literature review. Int J Child Comput Interact. Mar 2022;31:100421. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Powell L, Parker J, Harpin V. What is the level of evidence for the use of currently available technologies in facilitating the self-management of difficulties associated with ADHD in children and young people? A systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Nov 2018;27(11):1391-1412. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Romero-Ayuso D, Toledano-González A, Rodríguez-Martínez MD, Arroyo-Castillo P, Triviño-Juárez JM, González P, et al. Effectiveness of virtual reality-based interventions for children and adolescents with ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Children (Basel). Jan 21, 2021;8(2):70. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Higgins JP, Altman DG, Gøtzsche PC, Jüni P, Moher D, Oxman AD, Cochrane Bias Methods Group; et al. Cochrane Statistical Methods Group. The Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ. Oct 18, 2011;343(oct18 2):d5928. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. Jul 21, 2009;6(7):e1000097. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Twisk J, Proper K. Evaluation of the results of a randomized controlled trial: how to define changes between baseline and follow-up. J Clin Epidemiol. Mar 2004;57(3):223-228. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hox J, Moerbeek M, van de Schoot R. Multilevel Analysis: Techniques and Applications. 3rd edition. Oxfordshire, UK. Routledge; 2002.
- Corkum P, Elik N, Blotnicky-Gallant PA, McGonnell M, McGrath P. Web-based intervention for teachers of elementary students with ADHD: randomized controlled trial. J Atten Disord. Feb 2019;23(3):257-269. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Schlamp D, Kratz O, Studer P, et al. Neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: 6-month follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Sep 2010;19(9):715-724. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kollins SH, DeLoss DJ, Cañadas E, Lutz J, Findling RL, Keefe RS, et al. A novel digital intervention for actively reducing severity of paediatric ADHD (STARS-ADHD): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet Digit Health. Apr 2020;2(4):e168-e178. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Fried R, DiSalvo M, Kelberman C, Adler A, McCafferty D, Woodworth KY, et al. An innovative SMS intervention to improve adherence to stimulants in children with ADHD: preliminary findings. J Psychopharmacol. Aug 2020;34(8):883-890. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Brehmer Y, Westerberg H, Bäckman L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front Hum Neurosci. Mar 27, 2012;6:63. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hardy KK, Willard VW, Bonner MJ. Computerized cognitive training in survivors of childhood cancer: a pilot study. J Pediatr Oncol Nurs. Jan 2011;28(1):27-33. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Park K, Kihl T, Park S, Kim MJ, Chang J. Fairy tale directed game-based training system for children with ADHD using BCI and motion sensing technologies. Behav Inf Technol. 2018;38(6):564-577. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Ra CK, Cho J, Stone MD, De La Cerda J, Goldenson NI, Moroney E, et al. Association of digital media use with subsequent symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents. JAMA. Jul 17, 2018;320(3):255-263. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Mayer RE, Parong J, Bainbridge K. Young adults learning executive function skills by playing focused video games. Cogn Dev. Jan 2019;49:43-50. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Wexler BE, Vitulano LA, Moore C, Katsovich L, Smith SD, Rush C, et al. An integrated program of computer-presented and physical cognitive training exercises for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychol Med. Jul 2021;51(9):1524-1535. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kotwal DB, Burns WJ, Montgomery DD. Computer-assisted cognitive training for ADHD: a case study. Behav Modif. 1996;20(1):85-96. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Ordikhani-Seyedlar M, Lebedev MA, Sorensen HB, Puthusserypady S. Neurofeedback therapy for enhancing visual attention: state-of-the-art and challenges. Front Neurosci. Aug 03, 2016;10:352. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Leins U, Goth G, Hinterberger T, Klinger C, Rumpf N, Strehl U. Neurofeedback for children with ADHD: a comparison of SCP and Theta/Beta protocols. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. Jun 2007;32(2):73-88. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lofthouse N, Arnold LE, Hersch S, Hurt E, DeBeus R. A review of neurofeedback treatment for pediatric ADHD. J Atten Disord. Jul 2012;16(5):351-372. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gomes H, Duff M, Ramos M, Molholm S, Foxe JJ, Halperin J. Auditory selective attention and processing in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. Feb 2012;123(2):293-302. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Liu T, Wang J, Chen Y, Wang R, Song M. Neurofeedback treatment experimental study for ADHD by using the brain-computer interface neurofeedback system. In: Proceedings of the 2012 World Congress on Medical Physics and Biomedical Engineering. Presented at: WCMPBE '12; May 26-31, 2012, 2012;1537-1540; Beijing, China. URL: https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-642-29305-4_404 [CrossRef]
- Pickens TA, Khan SP, Berlau DJ. White noise as a possible therapeutic option for children with ADHD. Complement Ther Med. Feb 2019;42:151-155. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Clarfield J, Stoner G. The effects of computerized reading instruction on the academic performance of students identified with ADHD. Sch Psychol Rev. 2019;34(2):246-254. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef]
- Chacko A, Newcorn JH, Feirsen N, Uderman JZ. Improving medication adherence in chronic pediatric health conditions: a focus on ADHD in youth. Curr Pharm Des. Jul 01, 2010;16(22):2416-2423. [CrossRef] [Medline]
Abbreviations
ADHD: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder |
ADHD-RS: ADHD Rating Scale |
BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function |
CAVLT-2: Children’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test-2 |
Conners 3-P: Conners 3 Parent Rating Scale |
Conners 3-T: Conners 3 Teacher Rating Scale |
DBDRS: Disruptive Behavior Disorder Rating Scale |
IVA/CPT: Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test |
PedsQL: Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory |
PRISMA: Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses |
RCT: randomized controlled trial |
SMD: standardized mean difference |
WISC-IV: Backward Digit Span of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV |
Edited by J Torous; submitted 01.08.23; peer-reviewed by HF Hsieh, M Sakata; comments to author 14.09.23; revised version received 05.10.23; accepted 22.10.23; published 21.11.23.
Copyright©Ka Po Wong, Jing Qin, Yao Jie Xie, Bohan Zhang. Originally published in JMIR Mental Health (https://mental.jmir.org), 21.11.2023.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Mental Health, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://mental.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.