Original Paper
Abstract
Background: Emotional clarity has often been assessed with self-report measures, but efforts have also been made to measure it passively, which has advantages such as avoiding potential inaccuracy in responses stemming from social desirability bias or poor insight into emotional clarity. Response times (RTs) to emotion items administered in ecological momentary assessments (EMAs) may be an indirect indicator of emotional clarity. Another proposed indicator is the drift rate parameter, which assumes that, aside from how fast a person responds to emotion items, the measurement of emotional clarity also requires the consideration of how careful participants were in providing responses.
Objective: This paper aims to examine the reliability and validity of RTs and drift rate parameters from EMA emotion items as indicators of individual differences in emotional clarity.
Methods: Secondary data analysis was conducted on data from 196 adults with type 1 diabetes who completed a 2-week EMA study involving the completion of 5 to 6 surveys daily. If lower RTs and higher drift rates (from EMA emotion items) were indicators of emotional clarity, we hypothesized that greater levels (ie, higher clarity) should be associated with greater life satisfaction; lower levels of neuroticism, depression, anxiety, and diabetes distress; and fewer difficulties with emotion regulation. Because prior literature suggested emotional clarity could be valence specific, EMA items for negative affect (NA) and positive affect were examined separately.
Results: Reliability of the proposed indicators of emotional clarity was acceptable with a small number of EMA prompts (ie, 4 to 7 prompts in total or 1 to 2 days of EMA surveys). Consistent with expectations, the average drift rate of NA items across multiple EMAs had expected associations with other measures, such as correlations of r=–0.27 (P<.001) with depression symptoms, r=–0.27 (P=.001) with anxiety symptoms, r=–0.15 (P=.03) with emotion regulation difficulties, and r=0.63 (P<.001) with RTs to NA items. People with a higher NA drift rate responded faster to NA emotion items, had greater subjective well-being (eg, fewer depression symptoms), and had fewer difficulties with overall emotion regulation, which are all aligned with the expectation for an emotional clarity measure. Contrary to expectations, the validities of average RTs to NA items, the drift rate of positive affect items, and RTs to positive affect items were not strongly supported by our results.
Conclusions: Study findings provided initial support for the validity of NA drift rate as an indicator of emotional clarity but not for that of other RT-based clarity measures. Evidence was preliminary because the sample size was not sufficient to detect small but potentially meaningful correlations, as the sample size of the diabetes EMA study was chosen for other more primary research questions. Further research on passive emotional clarity measures is needed.
doi:10.2196/58352
Keywords
Introduction
Background
Emotion regulation is highly relevant to subjective well-being from both hedonic and eudaimonic perspectives. According to the hedonic perspective, well-being is the experience of happiness or the occurrence of positive affect (PA) and absence of negative affect (NA) [
]. In the eudaimonic perspective, well-being arises when individuals live with a sense of growth, meaningfulness, and purpose [ ]. People often engage in emotion regulation to obtain hedonic benefits (eg, feel more PA and reduce NA) [ ], but eudaimonic motivations have been found to be important as well [ ] (eg, downregulation of negative emotions to achieve a sense of growth in one’s ability to handle daily stressors).An important aspect of emotion regulation is emotional clarity, a person’s ability to lucidly identify the type of emotion they are experiencing [
]. Emotional clarity is highly relevant to the James Gross Model of Emotion Regulation [ ]. The model consists of emotion regulation strategies that are either “antecedent-focused” or “response-focused,” which refers to whether the strategy is used before or after an emotional response fully develops. In the updated emotion regulation model of Gross, understanding and identifying one’s emotions accurately (ie, emotional clarity) are precursors to both these types of emotion regulation strategies [ ]. Therefore, individuals with low emotional clarity are less likely to use emotion regulation strategies (as they are failing to identify the need for them), which can negatively impact well-being. Lower emotional clarity has often been associated with reduced mental health [ - ], although there are exceptions. For instance, prior research has suggested that higher emotional clarity may be adaptive primarily for individuals who do not have very frequent and strong experiences of negative emotions but maladaptive for those who frequently have strong feelings of NA [ ].Both direct and indirect measures of emotional clarity have been developed [
, ]. Direct assessments involve the metacognitive task of reflecting on one’s emotional clarity level, while indirect assessments measure the performance of a task relevant to emotional clarity (ie, answering emotion items) and do not require self-insight [ ]. Emotional clarity is commonly directly assessed with cross-sectional self-report measures such as the clarity subscales of the Trait Meta-Mood Scale [ ] and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) [ ], but indirect measures have been argued to have potential advantages over self-report assessments [ , ]. For instance, they could help avoid possible issues with subjective reports, including poor insight into emotional clarity and the possibility of social desirability bias (ie, participants not wanting to report uncertainty about their feelings) [ , ]. However, for the potential utility to be realized, further investigations of the validity of indirect measures of emotional clarity are needed.Assessing Emotional Clarity With Item Response Times
Response times (RTs) to emotion questions in ecological momentary assessments (EMAs) have been argued to be indirect measures of emotional clarity at the momentary (within-person) level [
]. Theoretically, the greater an individual’s momentary affective clarity, the less time should be needed to provide a rating of momentary affect [ ], whereas longer RTs to affect items should be indicative of lower emotional clarity. Evidence supporting this theory has been found, such as shorter RTs to affect items being associated with better momentary emotion regulation and mood [ ]. However, emotional clarity may be confounded with emotional intensity [ ], and evidence suggests the validity of RTs as a measure of affective clarity is enhanced by controlling for emotional intensity at the within-person (and not between-person) level [ ].There was no strong evidence that the study-long aggregates of emotion item RTs (between-person level) could act as indicators of trait emotional clarity [
, ]. These aggregates were found to not have significant relationships with global measures of emotional clarity and have inconsistent relationships with global emotion regulation measures [ , ]. Suggested reasons for the lack of a relationship between global measures of emotional clarity and the aggregate of emotion item RTs include a modality difference (ie, self-report vs indirect behavior-based assessment), low conceptual correspondence (ie, different forms of emotional clarity are being assessed), and difference in assessment timing (ie, 1 time vs repeated EMA measurements) [ , ]. The relationships between the study-long aggregate of emotion item RTs and subjective well-being variables of relevance to emotional clarity (eg, depression and anxiety) [ , ] were not examined, which could have served as useful additional convergent validity tests. Finally, the possibility that processing speed had a confounding effect on correlations between study-long aggregates of emotion item RTs and other variables was not investigated. In prior research, RTs to emotion EMA items have been found to have moderate correlations with processing speed measures [ ], suggesting that individual differences in emotion item RTs may at least in part be attributable to processing speed.Assessing Emotional Clarity With the Drift Rate Parameter
Another indirect measure of emotional clarity was recently proposed, drift rate, which is computed using the drift-diffusion model [
]. This model, which is often used in cognitive psychology, was explicitly developed to disentangle different components of RTs [ ]. The drift-diffusion model proposes that decisions (eg, choosing responses on EMA items) are made through the accumulation of information until a threshold of sufficient information (as determined by the individual) is reached [ ]. Decisions can be fast if the speed of information accumulation (ie, drift rate) is fast, if the threshold for a decision (ie, boundary separation or response caution) is low, or both. The D-diffusion item response theory (IRT) model is an IRT version of the drift-diffusion model that was specifically designed for the analysis of self-report ratings, such as emotion ratings [ ]. In the context of answering mood questions, the D-diffusion IRT model additionally considers that RTs may also be fast if extremes of the emotion are experienced (ie, very high or low levels), making the provision of ratings more straightforward [ ]. When the D-diffusion model is applied to EMA responses and their RTs, it can take into account all the aforementioned influences on RTs and output drift rate. In the EMA context, drift rate can be interpreted as the speed of access to information relevant to the question being asked [ , ], which here is information regarding emotions. A more detailed description of how the drift rate parameter was computed can be found in the Emotional Clarity Measure 2: Drift Rate From the Drift-Diffusion Model subsection in the Methods section.A person would be considered to have high emotional clarity when responding both fast and carefully to emotion items, which differs from how emotional clarity assessed via RTs considers only speed. As the absolute difference between a person’s NA level and the level of NA captured by an item increases, the D-diffusion model assumes that an individual would be expected to have faster RTs as a result of the so-called “distance-difficulty” principle, a well-established phenomenon in the RT literature [
], whereby the more items contrast with a person’s state, the easier they are. For instance, an individual with a very low NA would be expected to quickly respond to an item asking about being scared (an item often associated with a high NA) [ ], whereas the same individual would be expected to require a longer RT for an item asking about irritability (which has been associated with a low NA) [ ]. According to the D-diffusion model, response caution is low when a person responds similarly quickly (or slowly) to all items regardless of their content (an indication that the person did not answer carefully). Assuming a particular observed item RT, when response caution is lower (ie, less information is gathered), drift rate (ie, the rate of information accumulation or information divided by time) would also be lower. Conversely, for a person with a higher response caution, the estimated drift rate (and emotional clarity) would be greater for the same RT.Our initial evidence suggested that the drift rate parameter derived from EMA NA items may be an indicator of emotional clarity of low versus high NA, including expected relationships (ie, negative associations) at the between-person level with neuroticism and depression [
], but we did not account for factors aside from emotional clarity that could impact drift rate. For instance, drift rate could be impacted by individual differences in the general speed of responding to questionnaires (eg, due to reading speed, motor behavior, and familiarity with computer use). Studies on emotional clarity measured by survey item RTs had accounted for individual differences in this baseline speed of responding by adjusting for it [ ], but this was not done in our prior study [ ]. Cognitive processing speed was hypothesized to also potentially impact drift rate but was also not adjusted for, as the measure was not available in the prior study [ ].Objectives
Trait Emotional Clarity
There has been substantial prior research and interest in examining emotional clarity as a trait [
, , ], but further research on the indirect (instead of direct) measurement of individual differences in emotional clarity via EMA data is needed. Compared to traditional cross-sectional self-report measures, indirect measurement of individual differences in emotional clarity potentially has the advantage of less susceptibility to self-report biases stemming from causes such as social desirability and poor insight [ , ]. Furthermore, if an emotion is being assessed via EMA, indirect assessment of emotional clarity via emotion items affords the possibility of capturing individual differences in emotional clarity without burdening participants with additional items. The use of EMA methodology is ubiquitous in a broad range of fields [ - ], and the use of EMA emotion items is commonplace [ ]. Indirect assessment of trait emotional clarity would, therefore, make the investigation of emotional clarity possible for a large number of EMA data sets without additional emotional clarity items.The focus of this paper was to examine the reliability and validity of 2 RT-based indirect indicators of individual differences in emotional clarity among adults with type 1 diabetes (T1D). One indicator was the average of repeated measures of RTs to emotion items, and the other was the average of repeated measures of drift rate (ie, the speed of accessing information about one’s current affect) derived from emotion items. In validity testing, in contrast to prior work [
], individual differences in the baseline speed of responding and processing speed were controlled for. We examined data from an EMA study of adults with T1D [ ]. EMA surveys (which included emotion items) were completed 5 to 6 times a day (depending on the participants’ sleep schedule) for 2 weeks, and the RTs for each item were recorded. Notably, RT-based metrics can vary within people, and their study-long averages can vary across people. Multilevel modeling was used to account for both these potential sources of variance.Emotional clarity may be of particular relevance for adults with T1D. Compared to the general population, adults with T1D may be exposed to stressors more frequently, specifically emotional distress related to the daily care of diabetes. A prior study estimated that performing all recommended diabetes self-management activities would require >2 hours daily [
]. Given such burden and associated diabetes distress (emotional distress specific to the daily care of diabetes), individuals with diabetes are more likely to experience lower subjective well-being, such as more symptoms of depression, compared to healthy populations [ ]. Diabetes distress has been found to be negatively associated with emotion regulation ability [ ]. Therefore, emotional clarity, a possible precursor to emotion regulation [ ], may be integral for adults with T1D to cope with the burden of the condition. Poor coping with diabetes can lead to the neglect of diabetes self-management behaviors, which can amplify the health consequences of diabetes. Greater diabetes distress has been associated with lower adherence to insulin regimens [ , ], while greater depressive symptoms have been associated with lower adherence to diet, exercise, and glucose testing recommendations [ ].Reliability
The reliability of the proposed measures for emotional clarity was investigated by examining the test-retest stability of the measures across EMA measurement occasions.
Validity
The validity of the RT-based indicators of emotional clarity was tested by examining their associations with well-validated measures of relevance to emotional clarity. In forming our hypotheses (summarized in
), we made a distinction between the clarity of PA and the clarity of NA because of prior research suggesting that the latter had associations with mental health while the former did not [ ]. Therefore, we speculated that the awareness of NA is a more direct precursor to the application of coping strategies and successful coping than the awareness of PA.Emotional clarity indicator | |||||
NA drift rate | PA drift rate | RTa for NA itemsb | RT for PA items | ||
Subjective well-being | |||||
Satisfaction with life | Positive | Null | Positive | Null | |
Neuroticism | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Depression | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Anxiety | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Diabetes distress | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Emotion regulation and its 6 components | |||||
Emotion regulation difficulties (overall) | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Limited strategies | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Nonacceptance | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Impulse control difficulties | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Difficulties with goal directedness | Negative | Null | Negative | Null | |
Lack of awareness | Negative | Negative | Negative | Negative | |
Lack of emotional clarity | Negative | Negative | Negative | Negative |
aRT: response time.
bMultiplied by –1 so that higher values indicate greater emotional clarity.
Hypothesized Associations With Subjective Well-Being
Past literature has found that increased emotional clarity and emotion regulation ability, as assessed by questionnaires or RTs to emotion items, were associated with greater subjective well-being, including greater life satisfaction [
], lower neuroticism [ ], lower depression [ ], fewer anxiety symptoms [ ], and less diabetes distress [ ]. However, following the results of the study by Thompson et al [ ], we hypothesized that these relationships should hold only for indicators of NA clarity, not PA clarity.Hypothesized Associations With Emotion Regulation
We considered the valence of emotions in the generation of our hypotheses regarding associations between passively collected indicators of emotional clarity and emotion regulation, given previous work showing that the clarity of positive emotions had different associations with other measures (eg, neuroticism and depression) compared to the clarity of negative emotions [
]. The DERS [ ] has 6 subscales representative of emotion regulation components (listed in ), as well as an overall score. Of the 6 DERS subscales, 4 (67%) specifically assess problems with regulating NA (ie, limited access to emotion regulation strategies when upset, nonacceptance of negative emotional responses, impulse control difficulties when upset, and difficulties with goal-directed behaviors when upset); we hypothesized that greater difficulties indicated on each of these component scales would be associated with lower NA clarity on the proposed RT-based measures [ ], whereas we did not expect them to be associated with PA clarity on the RT-based measures ( ). That is, we expected NA clarity to precede and hence be more relevant to NA regulation than PA clarity, consistent with prior findings of associations between neuroticism and NA clarity, but not PA clarity [ ]. The remaining 2 (33%) components of emotion regulation (ie, a lack of emotional awareness and lack of emotional clarity) are not specific to the regulation of NA [ ]; therefore, we hypothesized that they would be associated with the clarity of both NA and PA using the RT-based measures. Notably, 1 of these 2 components assesses self-reported emotional clarity; we expected that this component would have associations of a greater magnitude with RT-based measures of NA and PA clarity compared to other emotion regulation subscales. Finally, we hypothesized an association between the DERS total score and indicators of NA clarity but not indicators of PA clarity because 4 (67%) of the 6 DERS subscales were relevant to emotion regulation when experiencing NA.Adjustment by Individual Differences in RT and Processing Speed
We examined whether adjusting the RT-based emotional clarity indicators by individual differences in RT and participant processing speed would affect the results of the convergent or divergent validity tests. Both RTs to survey items and drift rate were expected to not be purely indicators of emotional clarity but likely also be impacted by processing speed [
, , ] and differences in the baseline speed of responding [ ]. Therefore, we examined the robustness of the results of validity testing when statistically adjusting for both these variables.Methods
Overview
The analyzed data were from an EMA study focused on investigating the relationships among momentary blood glucose level, emotional state, and functioning in adults with T1D [
]. Participants were recruited from 3 clinical sites through health care provider referrals, mailings, flyers, and emails. Inclusion criteria were having a diagnosis of T1D, being able to speak and read English or Spanish, and the ability and willingness to carry out study procedures (eg, completion of EMAs and cognitive tests on smartphones). Study procedures included the completion of baseline surveys, training in the use of study devices, 2 weeks of 5 to 6 EMAs and ambulatory cognitive tests daily, and follow-up surveys. EMA surveys began at participants’ selected wake-up time each day and were administered at 3-hour intervals after that until sleep time. If a participant reported that they would likely be sleeping by the time of the sixth survey (ie, 15 hours after the first survey), then they were given the option to complete 5 surveys daily instead of 6. To encourage EMA compliance, 3 brief check-in emails or calls were scheduled with study staff. In addition, if EMA survey completion was low, then study staff would contact the participant to query if any support was required. The study procedures are described in greater depth in a protocol paper [ ].Ethical Considerations
All study procedures were approved by the University of Southern California’s Institutional Review Board (proposal #HS-18-01014). Informed consent was provided before study participation. Participants were compensated US $200 via a reloadable debit card for completion of all study procedures. Study data were deidentified before analysis.
Measures
RT-Based Measures of Emotional Clarity
The clarity of PA was assessed with RTs to EMA items about how happy, content, enthusiastic, or excited participants felt “right now,” while the clarity of NA was assessed with RTs to EMA items asking how tense, upset, sad, or disappointed participants felt in the moment. These emotion adjectives were taken from the “Stress and Working Memory” study [
] and were chosen because they mapped neatly onto the circumplex model of affect [ ]. That is, there were 2 items in each of the 4 circumplex dimensions (ie, unpleasant and activated, unpleasant and deactivated, pleasant and activated, and pleasant and deactivated), thereby ensuring that a range of emotion types was represented. The responses were all given on slider scales from 0 (not at all) to 100 (extremely). These emotion items were administered at every EMA prompt using the mobile EMA app (ilumivu [ ]). Items were presented one at a time on study-provided smartphones. For each item, RTs were recorded in milliseconds. RTs that were deemed too fast (ie, <0.2 seconds) or too slow (ie, >30 seconds) were set to missing for analyses (5979/461,896, 1.29% of the observations) because such outliers could be indicative of careless responding or distractions during survey completion [ , ].Emotional Clarity Measure 1: Median RTs
NA and PA clarity were computed as the median RT of the 4 NA items and the 4 PA items at each EMA prompt [
, ] (RTs were multiplied by –1 such that higher values indicate greater clarity). In this paper, median RT for NA items will be referred to as NA RT and median RT for PA items as PA RT.Following prior research, the median RTs were adjusted for the baseline speed of responding to partial out individual differences in RT stemming from factors such as reading and screen tapping speed [
, ]. We assessed participants’ baseline speed by taking the median RT across all EMA occasions on a multiple-choice question asking what they were doing immediately before the survey.Emotional Clarity Measure 2: Drift Rate From the Drift-Diffusion Model
Drift rates for both NA and PA items were calculated for each EMA following procedures described in detail in a prior study [
] with software code available [ ]. In brief, we estimated the drift rates for each person and EMA measurement occasion using the IRT based-variant of the drift-diffusion model that was specifically developed for use with self-report (eg, EMA) items. Because the IRT model requires binary variables, responses to PA and NA items were converted into dichotomous variables such that responses below the midpoint of the scale were coded as 0, while responses at or above the midpoint of the scale were coded as 1. Previous analyses have shown that drift rate measures derived from continuous items that were dichotomized demonstrate convergent validity with items that were already presented to respondents in a binary response format [ ]. Next, we examined whether the dichotomized PA and NA item sets were unidimensional, a condition necessary for the calculation of drift rate; we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis in Mplus (version 8.8; Muthén & Muthén) [ ] using the weighted least square mean and variance adjusted estimator, using cluster-robust SEs to account for the nesting of multiple EMA measurement occasions within individuals. We examined whether fit indices were within the traditional ranges for acceptable model fit, including a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of at least <0.08, comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) of at least >0.90, and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) of <0.08 [ ]. Within and between-person reliability (McDonald omega) coefficients were also computed for the dichotomized and nondichotomized NA and PA items [ ].A drift-diffusion model was then applied to the RTs and dichotomized response values for PA and NA items using the diffIRT package in R (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) [
], where drift rate and RTs were modeled as latent factors. Factor score estimates for the drift rate parameter were then calculated for each EMA occasion, separately for NA and PA. To examine the fit of the drift-diffusion model on the PA and NA item sets, we examined the level of consistency between the observed and diffusion model–predicted RT distributions with histograms and density plots.The drift rate parameters used in our primary analyses are a processed version of the drift rate parameters from the diffusion IRT models referred to as the absolute drift rate, computed as the mean absolute difference between the drift rate parameter factor scores and the item difficulty levels. The drift rate parameter is an estimation of a person’s tendency to report high NA (or PA) in a moment after taking into account both a person’s responses and item RTs. However, emotional clarity should be indicated by the speed in carefully accessing one’s mood regardless of its valence (eg, high or low NA). Therefore, following the distance-difficulty hypothesis-informed formula for the speed of information accumulation in the D-diffusion model [
], we found the absolute value of the difference between the drift rate parameter and average item difficulty and operationalized this absolute drift rate as emotional clarity. These absolute drift rates were then log transformed to normalize their distributions. When used in analyses, drift rates were also adjusted by baseline speed.Other Measures
The measures used for convergent or divergent validity testing were completed before (“baseline”) or after (“follow-up”) the EMA study period. Life satisfaction was assessed with the Satisfaction with Life Scale [
], neuroticism with the Ten-Item Personality Inventory [ ], depression with the Patient Health Questionnaire [ ], anxiety with the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale [ ], diabetes distress with the Problem Areas in Diabetes Scale short form [ ], and emotion regulation with the DERS short form (DERS-SF) [ ]. The Ten-Item Personality Inventory, Patient Health Questionnaire, Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale, and DERS-SF were administered at baseline, while the Satisfaction with Life Scale and Problem Areas in Diabetes Scale short form were completed at follow-up [ ].Processing speed was assessed with the Symbol Search task [
], an ambulatory cognitive test administered as part of every EMA prompt [ ]. The Symbol Search task captured perceptual speed, which is a component of processing speed [ , ]. Participants were presented with 2 cards at the top and 2 cards at the bottom of the phone screen, each with 2 symbols. As quickly as they could, they were asked to choose the card at the bottom of the screen that matched with one of the cards on top. The task consisted of 20 trials, and processing speed was measured as the median RT in accurate trials, only for sessions with at least 70% (14/20) matching accuracy [ ]. Symbol Search RTs were calculated such that higher values indicate faster processing speed.Statistical Analyses
Reliability
Reliability was assessed for each of the emotional clarity indicators: NA drift rate, PA drift rate, NA RT, and PA RT. It was calculated using the following formula: between-person reliability=variance (between-person)/(variance [between-person] + variance [within-person]/n) [
], where between-person variance is the variance in the average of scores across measurement occasions (ie, EMA prompts), within-person variance is the variance of scores across measurement occasions within a person, and n is the number of measurement occasions. Between and within-person variance were calculated with multilevel models, with EMA prompts nested in individuals, where the measure of interest (eg, the NA drift rate) was specified at both levels 1 and 2. To examine how many measurement occasions would be needed to obtain acceptable reliability (≥0.7) for each emotional clarity indicator, we estimated how reliability changed as a function of the number of measurement occasions, moving from 2 EMA prompts to a maximum of 70 prompts. Mplus (version 8.10) [ ] was used for reliability analyses via the package MplusAutomation [ ] in the statistical software R (The R Foundation) [ ].Validity
Validity testing was performed for average NA drift rate, PA drift rate, NA RT, and PA RT. To account for the nested data structure, with multiple EMA prompts nested in individuals, we estimated correlation coefficients between the emotional clarity indicators and other measures using multilevel structural equation models (MSEMs). Multilevel variables with both within- and between-person variance (NA drift rate, PA drift rate, NA RT, PA RT, and processing speed) were specified at both level 1 (within-person) and level 2 (between-person), with latent means estimated at level 2 of the MSEM. As all cross-sectional measures only contained between-person variance, they were entered into the MSEM at level 2 and allowed to correlate with the NA drift rate, PA drift rate, NA RT, and PA RT variables. To adjust for individual differences in baseline speed, NA drift rate, PA drift rate, NA RT, and PA RT were regressed on baseline speed at level 2 of the MSEM. In addition, covariances were specified between baseline speed and all other level-2 variables. Prior research indicated that RTs to tasks were affected by the time of day [
, ]. Therefore, at level 1, all the RT-based metrics were adjusted for (ie, regressed on) the time of day, coded as a categorical variable, where a participant’s first survey per day was categorized as taking place in the morning, their final scheduled survey of the day as taking place in the evening, and all their surveys in between as taking place midday. The reference group was “midday,” meaning that at level 2, the latent means for the RT-based metrics were for midday surveys.Various sensitivity tests were also conducted. Separate multilevel regression models explored whether adjustment for emotional intensity impacted the relationships between RT and each cross-sectional measure, such that NA RT and PA RT were additionally regressed on linear and quadratic terms of overall NA and PA ratings (ie, average rating across NA or PA items within an EMA survey), respectively. In supplemental analyses in which all the proposed emotional clarity indicators were controlled for processing speed, they were all regressed on processing speed at level 2. Finally, we tested whether the association between NA and PA drift rate and other measures would differ if the drift metrics were computed from mood items that were dichotomized at each person’s mean NA and PA response rather than the scale midpoint. For all analyses, data from all participants were included regardless of completion rates because MSEMs estimate latent averages of variables at level 1 that account for potential unreliability stemming from sparse participant data [
]. All validity analyses were conducted in Mplus (version 8.10) [ ] using maximum likelihood with robust SEs. Code for both the reliability and validity analyses is provided [ ].Results
Descriptive Statistics
Characteristics of the study sample, 196 adults with T1D, are shown in
. The median EMA completion rate over the 2-week study period was 92% (IQR 11%).Descriptive statistics for the EMA variables are presented in
. Distributions for RTs to individual NA and PA items are shown in Figures S1 and S2 in , respectively. Tables S1 and S2 in show the between-person correlations between (unadjusted) study measures.A unidimensional model was found to fit both the (dichotomized) 4 NA and 4 PA items acceptably, justifying the calculation of drift rates for both types of items. For NA, χ22=10.6, P=.005; CFI=0.998; TLI=0.994; RMSEA=0.018; and SRMR=0.018, while for PA, χ22=53.3, P<.001; CFI=0.989; TLI=0.966; RMSEA=0.044; and SRMR=0.031. All these values were within commonly suggested ranges for acceptable model fit [
]. The within-person omega estimate for the 4 dichotomized NA items was 0.710, while the between-person omega estimate was 0.938. For the 4 dichotomized PA items, the within-person omega estimate was 0.674, and the between-person omega estimate was 0.939. The within-person omega estimate for the 4 nondichotomized NA items was 0.793, while the between-person omega estimate was 0.955. For the 4 nondichotomized PA items, the within-person omega estimate was 0.773, and the between-person omega estimate was 0.937. For all PA and NA items, the observed RT distributions (histograms) were consistent with the drift-diffusion model–predicted RT distributions as per density plots of these RTs (Figures S1 and S2 in ), indicating a good fit overall for the D-diffusion IRT model [ ].Characteristic | Values | ||
Age (y), mean (SD; range) | 39.6 (14.3; 18-75) | ||
Sex n (%) | |||
Male | 88 (44.9) | ||
Female | 108 (55.1) | ||
Other | 0 (0) | ||
Ethnicity, n (%) | |||
African American | 29 (14.8) | ||
Asian | 7 (3.6) | ||
Latino | 80 (40.8) | ||
White | 56 (28.6) | ||
Multiethnic | 14 (7.1) | ||
Other | 6 (3.1) | ||
Not reported | 4 (2) | ||
Employment status, n (%) | |||
Full time | 69 (35.2) | ||
Part time | 23 (11.7) | ||
Full-time homemaker | 9 (4.6) | ||
Student | 18 (9.2) | ||
Unemployed | 27 (13.8) | ||
Retired | 15 (7.7) | ||
Disabled | 23 (11.7) | ||
Other | 8 (4.1) | ||
Not reported | 4 (2) | ||
Annual household income (US $), n (%) | |||
<25,000 | 47 (24) | ||
25,000-49,999 | 43 (21.9) | ||
50,000-74,999 | 15 (7.7) | ||
≥75,000 | 40 (20.4) | ||
Not provided | 51 (26) | ||
Mental healtha | |||
SWLSb score, mean (SD; range) | 22.0 (7.5; 5-35) | ||
TIPIc neuroticism score, mean (SD; range) | 3.2 (1.3; 1-7) | ||
PHQd score, mean (SD; range) | 5.4 (4.3; 0-24) | ||
PHQ score≥9 (moderate or higher depression), n (%) | 30 (15.3) | ||
GADe score, mean (SD; range) | 4.6 (3.8; 0-21) | ||
GAD score≥9 (moderate or higher anxiety), n (%) | 19 (9.7) | ||
PAIDf score, mean (SD; range) | 8.0 (5.5; 0-20) | ||
DERSg score, mean (SD; range) | 1.9 (0.6; 1-5) |
aPossible score ranges for the surveys were listed. Only for the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale, the observed score range (0-19) was different from the possible range.
bSWLS: Satisfaction with Life Scale.
cTIPI: Ten-Item Personality Inventory.
dPHQ: Patient Health Questionnaire.
eGAD: Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale.
fPAID: Problem Areas in Diabetes Scale assessing diabetes distress.
gDERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale.
EMA variable | All observations, mean (SD; range) | Between-person variance | Average within-person variance | ICCa | Between-person variance of log within-person variances |
NAb drift rate | –0.05 (0.84; –2.43 to 2.31) | 0.19 | 0.54 | 0.27 | 0.27 |
PAc drift rate | –0.53 (0.83; –1.68 to 2.05) | 0.19 | 0.51 | 0.28 | 0.18 |
NA RTd | 2.22 (1.70; 0.31 to 24.20) | 0.70 | 1.95 | 0.26 | 1.00 |
PA RT | 2.39 (1.83; 0.22 to 28.24) | 0.84 | 2.02 | 0.29 | 0.89 |
Sum of 4 NA items | 19.69 (20.26; 0 to 100) | 247.10 | 243.15 | 0.50 | 1.60 |
Sum of 4 PA items | 49.78 (23.87; 0 to 100) | 338.85 | 327.01 | 0.51 | 1.40 |
Sum of dichotomized NA items | 0.71 (1.21; 0 to 4) | 0.75 | 1.95 | 0.28 | 3.27 |
Sum of dichotomized PA items | 2.43 (1.50; 0 to 4) | 1.05 | 1.88 | 0.36 | 1.64 |
aICC: intraclass correlation coefficient.
bNA: negative affect.
cPA: positive affect.
dRT: response time.
Reliability
Figure S3 in
shows the between-person reliabilities as a function of the number of EMA prompts completed for each proposed emotional clarity indicator. For all indicators, reliability increased with more prompts, but each of the proposed emotional clarity indicators demonstrated acceptable reliability (ie, ≥0.70) with a relatively small number of EMA occasions. NA RT required 5 EMA prompts for acceptable reliability, and PA RT required 4 prompts. Both average NA and PA drift rates showed 0.70 reliability when 7 EMA prompts were completed.Validity
Relationships between NA RT and other measures were not consistent with our hypotheses. No significant associations were found with subjective well-being or emotion regulation measures (
; P values ranging from .08 to .98). PA RT was not significantly associated with any well-being or emotion regulation variable ( ; P values ranging from 25 to 93), except, unexpectedly, for diabetes distress (r=0.17; P=.009). After adjustment for processing speed, the associations between both NA RT and PA RT and diabetes distress were unexpectedly significant in a positive direction (r=0.14; P=.03 and r=0.20; P=.002, respectively). Adjustment for emotional intensity changed effects very minimally at the between-person level, consistent with the findings of a prior study [ ], so the results from that model were not reported here. Both NA RT and PA RT had significant associations with processing speed (r=0.25; P<.001 and r=0.21; P=.001, respectively).The associations between NA drift rate and other measures were consistent with our hypotheses overall. Unexpectedly, NA drift rate was not related to the lack of emotional clarity (P=.13) or 3 of the other DERS subscales (
; P values from .18 to .76). It was also not associated with life satisfaction (P=.17). However, a greater NA drift rate was significantly associated with lower neuroticism (r=–0.18; P=.01), depression (r=–0.27; P<.001), anxiety (r=–0.27; P<.001), and diabetes distress (r=–0.17; P=.005); lower overall difficulties with emotion regulation (total DERS score; r=–0.15; P=.03); less limited emotion regulation strategies (r=–0.15; P=.048); and lower lack of emotion awareness (r=–0.18; P=.009). Adjustment by processing speed resulted in minimal changes. When NA and PA drift rates were computed from mood items that were dichotomized at each person’s mean NA and PA ratings (rather than at the scale midpoint), the results showed mostly nonsignificant correlations with other measures (refer to Table S3 in ).NAa drift rate | NA drift rate, adjustedb | PAc drift rate | PA drift rate, adjustedb | NA RTd,e | NA RT, adjustedb,e | PA RTe | PA RT, adjustedb,e | ||||||||||||||||||||
Subjective well-being | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Satisfaction with life | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0.1 | 0.1 | –0.1 | –0.1 | –0.03 | –0.04 | –0.04 | –0.05 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .17 | .18 | .09 | .09 | .67 | .57 | .51 | .45 | |||||||||||||||||||
Neuroticism | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.18 | –0.18 | –0.08 | –0.08 | –0.06 | –0.06 | –0.01 | –0.01 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .01f | .01f | .29 | .29 | .51 | .44 | .91 | .85 | |||||||||||||||||||
Depression | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.27 | –0.27 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0 | 0 | 0.06 | 0.06 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | <.001f | <.001f | .73 | .73 | .98 | .99 | .47 | .45 | |||||||||||||||||||
Anxiety | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.27 | –0.27 | –0.09 | –0.09 | –0.09 | –0.09 | –0.03 | –0.03 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | <.001f | <.001f | .21 | .22 | .34 | .28 | .77 | .74 | |||||||||||||||||||
Diabetes distress | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.17 | –0.16 | 0.17 | 0.17 | 0.09 | 0.14 | 0.17 | 0.2 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .005f | .008f | .009f | .008f | .14 | .03f | .009f | .002f | |||||||||||||||||||
Emotion regulation | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
DERSg (total) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.15 | –0.16 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.03 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .03f | .01f | .78 | .78 | .64 | .92 | .39 | .58 | |||||||||||||||||||
Limited strategies | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.15 | –0.15 | 0.03 | 0.03 | –0.04 | –0.04 | –0.03 | –0.03 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .048f | .04f | .76 | .76 | .59 | .59 | .66 | .67 | |||||||||||||||||||
Nonacceptance | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.1 | –0.11 | 0.04 | 0.03 | –0.01 | –0.03 | 0 | –0.01 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .18 | .13 | .64 | .66 | .87 | .61 | .93 | .85 | |||||||||||||||||||
Impulse control difficulties | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.13 | 0.13 | 0.02 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 0.05 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .76 | .65 | .18 | .18 | .79 | .39 | .67 | .33 | |||||||||||||||||||
Difficulties with goal directedness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.07 | –0.08 | –0.11 | –0.11 | 0.11 | 0.09 | 0.08 | 0.06 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .32 | .26 | .20 | .18 | .08 | .13 | .25 | .36 | |||||||||||||||||||
Lack of awareness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.18 | –0.18 | –0.09 | –0.09 | –0.07 | –0.1 | –0.01 | –0.03 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .009f | .005f | .20 | .18 | .35 | .19 | .84 | .64 | |||||||||||||||||||
Lack of emotional clarity | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.11 | –0.12 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0 | –0.03 | 0.03 | 0 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .13 | .09 | .59 | .59 | .95 | .64 | .73 | .95 | |||||||||||||||||||
Processing speed (control variable) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0.09 | —h | 0.02 | — | 0.25 | — | 0.21 | — | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .18 | — | .78 | — | <.001f | — | <.001f | — |
aNA: negative affect.
bAdjusted for processing speed.
cPA: positive affect.
dRT: response time.
eMultiplied by –1 so that higher values indicate greater emotional clarity.
fP<.05.
gDERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale.
hNot applicable.
Consistent with expectations, PA drift rate was not significantly associated with most subjective well-being or NA-specific emotion regulation measures (
; P values from .09 to .76). Unexpectedly, PA drift rate was not significantly associated with emotional clarity (P=.59) or emotional awareness (P=.20) before or after adjustment for processing speed. Furthermore, contrary to expectations, a greater PA drift rate was associated with greater diabetes distress (r=0.17; P=.009). Neither NA drift rate nor PA drift rate had significant associations with processing speed (P=.18 and P=.78, respectively). Without adjustment for baseline speed, a few of the associations differed, such as the relationship between NA drift rate and life satisfaction (r=0.16; P=.02; ).NAa drift rate | NA drift rate, adjustedb | PAc drift rate | PA drift rate, adjustedb | NA RTd,e | NA RT, adjustedb,e | PA RTe | PA RT, adjustedb,e | ||||||||||||||||||||
Subjective well-being | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
Satisfaction with life | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0.16 | 0.14 | 0 | –0.03 | 0.1 | 0.06 | 0.09 | 0.05 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .02f | .05 | .96 | .67 | .15 | .40 | .15 | .42 | |||||||||||||||||||
Neuroticism | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.14 | –0.16 | –0.05 | –0.06 | –0.01 | –0.04 | 0.02 | 0 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .04f | .02f | .48 | .38 | .87 | .63 | .74 | .94 | |||||||||||||||||||
Depression | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.27 | –0.28 | –0.01 | 0 | –0.05 | –0.04 | –0.01 | 0 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | <.001f | <.001f | .92 | 1.00 | .43 | .57 | .85 | .95 | |||||||||||||||||||
Anxiety | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.25 | –0.26 | –0.08 | –0.09 | –0.07 | –0.08 | –0.03 | –0.03 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | <.001f | <.001f | .24 | .21 | .34 | .24 | .69 | .64 | |||||||||||||||||||
Diabetes distress | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.25 | –0.2 | 0.04 | 0.1 | –0.08 | 0.03 | –0.04 | 0.07 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | <.001f | .003f | .53 | .17 | .23 | .67 | .59 | .33 | |||||||||||||||||||
Emotion regulation | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
DERSg (total) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.15 | –0.17 | 0 | –0.01 | –0.01 | –0.04 | 0 | –0.02 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .045f | .009f | .97 | .92 | .91 | .51 | .95 | .70 | |||||||||||||||||||
Limited strategies | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.12 | –0.13 | 0.03 | 0.04 | –0.02 | –0.02 | –0.01 | –0.01 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .12 | .07 | .70 | .67 | .81 | .78 | .90 | .90 | |||||||||||||||||||
Nonacceptance | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.06 | –0.09 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 0.04 | –0.01 | 0.05 | 0 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .41 | .18 | .42 | .61 | .58 | .83 | .44 | .95 | |||||||||||||||||||
Impulse control difficulties | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0 | 0.03 | 0.09 | 0.12 | –0.02 | 0.04 | –0.02 | 0.04 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .99 | .67 | .36 | .21 | .79 | .48 | .82 | .47 | |||||||||||||||||||
Difficulties with goal directedness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.07 | –0.09 | –0.1 | –0.12 | 0.07 | 0.04 | 0.04 | 0.01 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .35 | .21 | .23 | .14 | .27 | .46 | .47 | .79 | |||||||||||||||||||
Lack of awareness | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.19 | –0.21 | –0.12 | –0.13 | –0.1 | –0.14 | –0.06 | –0.09 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .01f | .002f | .13 | .07 | .13 | .02f | .32 | .12 | |||||||||||||||||||
Lack of emotional clarity | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | –0.14 | –0.16 | –0.01 | –0.02 | –0.07 | –0.1 | –0.05 | –0.07 | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | .07 | .04f | .92 | .81 | .38 | .17 | .54 | .32 | |||||||||||||||||||
Processing speed (control variable) | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||
r | 0.28 | —h | 0.25 | — | 0.5 | — | 0.48 | — | |||||||||||||||||||
P value | <.001f | — | .005f | — | <.001f | — | <.001f | — |
aNA: negative affect.
bAdjusted for processing speed.
cPA: positive affect.
dRT: response time.
eMultiplied by –1 so that higher values indicate greater emotional clarity.
fP<.05.
gDERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale.
hNot applicable.
Discussion
Principal Findings
The most notable finding from this study was that the average NA drift rate, a proposed indicator of typical emotional clarity, had expected associations overall with validated measures of subjective well-being and emotion regulation, both before and after adjustment for processing speed and emotional intensity. By contrast, NA RT, another proposed indicator of typical emotional clarity, did not have the anticipated associations with the validated measures. Relative to NA drift rate, NA RT may be confounded by a greater number of factors aside from emotional clarity.
Nevertheless, there is no sufficient evidence to conclude that the average NA drift rate is a valid indicator of NA clarity, while the average NA RT is not a valid indicator of NA clarity due to sample size constraints. In post hoc power analyses, with our sample size of 196 participants, there was 80% power to detect a between-person correlation of 0.20. Therefore, the study may have been underpowered to detect small correlations that might still be meaningful. Furthermore, the sample size was not chosen a priori to be sufficiently powered to detect between-person relationships after adjustment for multiplicity of testing [
] because the sample size was conditioned on research questions that were more primary for the diabetes EMA study.While we cannot conclude that NA drift rate is a valid indicator of NA clarity, our results suggest that researchers in future studies should continue to investigate NA drift rate as an implicit measure of emotional clarity. We applied stringent validity tests by adjusting for both individual differences in the baseline speed of responding and processing speed and found significant associations between NA drift rate and 4 (80%) of the 5 subjective well-being ratings. Correlations with 4 (67%) of the 6 emotion regulation subscales were in the expected directions. Finally, it is notable that NA drift rate had a correlation of 0.63 (P<.001) with NA RT. People with a higher NA drift rate responded faster to the NA emotion items, had greater subjective well-being (eg, fewer depression symptoms), and had fewer difficulties with overall emotion regulation, which are all aligned with the expectation for an emotional clarity measure. Collectively, study results suggest that NA drift rate deserves further attention in future research.
Some of the magnitudes of correlations between NA drift rate and other study measures were comparable to sizes of correlations between formal (self-report) assessments of emotional clarity and other measures found in prior studies. One study found that in a group with generalized anxiety disorder, a self-report assessment of emotional clarity had correlations of –0.29 and –0.33 with depression and anxiety, respectively [
], which are similar to the correlations found in this study. In the same study, the association between emotional clarity and depression or anxiety was not significant in the healthy control group [ ], suggesting that the mental health status of the sample may affect the magnitude of the observed relationships. Other correlations found between emotional clarity and depression were –0.24 in a clinical sample [ ] and –0.29 in elementary school–aged children [ ]. Correlations between self-reported emotional clarity and neuroticism ranged between –0.31 in a sample of adolescents [ ] and –0.37 in a sample of college students [ ], and correlations between emotional clarity and life satisfaction ranged between 0.31 in adolescents [ ] and 0.35 in undergraduate students [ ], which are larger than the correlations observed in this study.When NA drift rate was computed from mood items that were dichotomized at each person’s mean response rather than the scale midpoint, no significant associations were observed with other study measures. The distance-difficulty hypothesis underlying the D-diffusion model that was used to generate the NA drift parameter assumes that people respond faster to items that contrast more with their current state [
]. According to this hypothesis, an individual with very low NA (regardless of their average level of NA) was expected to quickly report not being scared (an item often associated with high NA). Dichotomizing mood at each person’s own midpoint created a variable representing whether their mood was higher or lower relative to their personal average and not higher or lower in absolute terms (which could be approximated by dichotomizing at the scale midpoint). Perhaps, the former was less relevant to the distance-difficulty hypothesis compared to the latter because it captured relative mood and not actual mood, leading to the creation of NA drift parameters with no associations with other study measures.The reliabilities for the average NA drift rate, PA drift rate, NA RT, and PA RT were all acceptable with a small number of EMA prompts (ie, 4 to 7 EMA prompts or 1 to 2 days of EMA surveys). Therefore, reliable measurement of these proposed emotional clarity indicators would likely be feasible in most EMA studies where affect items are administered and RTs are recorded.
Secondary Findings
We found preliminary evidence supporting the argument that emotional clarity deficits are valence specific [
]. NA drift rate and PA drift rate had differential associations with self-report measures. Furthermore, they were moderately correlated with each other (r=0.38; refer to Table S1 in ). Had NA and PA drift rates been redundant with one another, a high correlation would have been expected.Greater PA drift rate and PA RT were unexpectedly found to be associated with higher diabetes distress and not associated with the awareness or clarity subscales of the DERS. It is unclear why people with greater diabetes distress would have greater clarity of positive emotions. Perhaps, when overwhelmed with burden from diabetes, people had a greater appreciation of positive emotional states and hence greater clarity of PA. One possible reason why PA drift rate and PA RT had far from significant associations with self-reported emotional clarity may have been because, given that other items in the DERS-SF asked questions relevant to the NA context, participants were primed to answer the emotional clarity questions with reference to feeling NA. More assessments of the validity of PA drift rate and PA RT are needed.
Future Directions
More conclusive evidence of the validity of RT-based measures of emotional clarity may come from studies where NA clarity can be manipulated, and the proposed emotional clarity indicators can be compared for sensitivity to these changes. For instance, people who undergo a mindfulness intervention may be expected to have higher NA clarity in the period following the intervention, and this effect should be reflected in changes in drift rates or RTs to NA EMA items.
EMA mood item RT–based measures of emotional clarity have a great potential utility. They can serve as indices of emotional clarity that do not require burdening participants with additional emotional clarity items. Furthermore, they can help avoid possible issues with subjective reports, including poor insight into emotional clarity and the possibility of social desirability bias [
]. Although the validity of the RT-based emotional clarity measures at the within-person level was not investigated in this study, such validity would allow for the investigation of changes in emotional clarity within and across days and the situational factors that contribute to them. For the potential utility of EMA mood item RT–based measures of emotional clarity to be realized, further investigations of the validity of EMA RT–based measures of emotional clarity (ie, at both the between-person and within-person levels) are needed.Limitations
We had decided not to adjust for multiple comparisons, but to also acknowledge that any results would require replication by future studies. It has been argued that the need for adjustment for multiple comparisons should be evaluated on a case-by-case basis and P value adjustment should not be used for all analyses [
]. For instance, adjustment for multiple comparisons comes with not only the benefit of lowering type 1 error but also the disadvantage of increasing the chance of type 2 error. Therefore, one factor to consider when deciding whether to adjust for multiple comparisons is the relative cost of type 1 and type 2 errors for a particular research question [ ]. In confirmatory studies with results that have implications for changes in clinical practice or the use of a treatment, the cost of a type 1 error may be higher than that of a type 2 error; hence, P value adjustment for multiple comparisons would be sensible [ ]. When performing post hoc analyses on existing data as part of theory building and testing (and without direct treatment implications), the relative cost of a type 2 error may be higher; hence, there may be a stronger argument for not using multiple comparisons adjustment [ ]. That is, a type 2 error could cause researchers to not detect potentially important findings [ ]. If adjustment is not used, there would need to be an acknowledgment that, to account for the possibility of a type 1 error, further research is needed to examine whether results can be replicated [ ].Nevertheless, we still tested the effect of false discovery rate adjustment on the P values of correlations for the different groups of hypotheses that were tested (eg, association between NA drift rate and subjective well-being measures). Adjustments for multiple comparisons are often applied separately for distinct families of hypotheses [
]. Tables S4 and S5 in show the false discovery rate–adjusted P values associated with and , respectively. The biggest differences to note were that several of the associations between NA drift rate and emotional regulation measures were no longer significant.Using the drift-diffusion model had the advantage of reducing the impact of individual differences in response caution (and potential careless responding) from emotion item RTs but had the disadvantage of assuming a 2-choice task (eg, a high vs low NA) underlying people’s emotion ratings. Because the drift-diffusion model required making the continuous PA and NA items dichotomous for data analysis, granular differences in emotional clarity may have been missed with the drift rate parameter.
We were unable to examine the within-person validity of RT-based clarity measures because EMA measures key to such testing (eg, self-reported emotional clarity and mood regulation success) [
] were not administered in this study. Future studies are needed to examine the within-person validity of the drift rate parameter as a within-person indicator of emotional clarity.The context in which RTs for emotion items were calculated was similar but not identical to that in prior work. For instance, in this study, median RTs for EMA were calculated based on RTs to 4 items. In prior studies, the median RTs for 5 to 8 items were computed [
, ]. In the original paper examining the validity of RTs to emotion items as indicators of emotional clarity, bipolar mood items were used (eg, items with options from “very unhappy” to “very happy”) [ ], whereas our study analyzed unipolar mood items. The results of this study may have been impacted to an extent by differences in EMA administration, such as variations in the type of emotion items used.Additional evidence is needed that study results generalize beyond adults with T1D. Other populations with an increased likelihood of experiencing lower subjective well-being (eg, individuals with various chronic conditions) [
] may be appropriate targets for future emotional clarity studies.Conclusions
A measure of NA drift rate derived from RTs to momentary NA items had expected associations with validated measures of relevance to emotional clarity, providing initial evidence supporting its validity as an indicator of individual differences in the clarity of negative emotions. The validities of NA RT, PA RT, and PA drift rate were not strongly supported by our results. More studies are needed to investigate the validities of NA and PA drift rate and NA and PA RT with larger sample sizes. The development of passive measures of emotional clarity would help create minimally burdensome measures of emotional clarity that are less vulnerable to possible issues from subjective self-reports, such as poor clarity insight and social desirability bias. Such measures may be useful in investigations relevant to the role of emotional clarity in people’s experience of well-being.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, National Institutes of Health, under grant number NIH/NIDDK 1R01DK121298-01, and the National Institute of Aging, National Institutes of Health, under grant number NIH/NIA R01 AG068190. The Southern California Clinical and Translational Science Institute (SC CTSI) supported the use of REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture; Vanderbilt University) in this study (NIH/NCATS UL1TR001855 and UL1TR000130). Additional support was provided by the New York Regional Center for Diabetes Translation Research (NIH/NIDDK P30 DK111022). The Symbol Search cognitive test was developed in a project supported by the National Institute on Aging at the National Institutes of Health (NIH/NIA #1U2CAG060408-01; principal investigator: Martin Sliwinski).
Data Availability
The data set analyzed for this study is available from the corresponding author, RH, on reasonable request.
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
Plots of between person reliability as a function of number of ecological momentary assessments and unadjusted between person correlations between study variables.
DOCX File , 807 KBReferences
- Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwarz N. Well-Being: Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. New York, NY. Russell Sage Foundation; 1999.
- Stone AA, Mackie C. Subjective Well-Being: Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience. Washington, DC. National Academies Press; 2013.
- Tamir M, Chiu CY, Gross JJ. Business or pleasure? Utilitarian versus hedonic considerations in emotion regulation. Emotion. Aug 2007;7(3):546-554. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ortner CN, Corno D, Fung TY, Rapinda K. The roles of hedonic and eudaimonic motives in emotion regulation. Pers Individ Differ. Jan 2018;120:209-212. [CrossRef]
- Coffey E, Berenbaum H, Kerns J. The dimensions of emotional intelligence, alexithymia, and mood awareness: associations with personality and performance on an emotional stroop task. Cogn Emot. Jul 18, 2003;17(4):671-679. [CrossRef]
- Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol. Sep 1998;2(3):271-299. [CrossRef]
- Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychol Inq. Mar 09, 2015;26(1):1-26. [CrossRef]
- Sendzik L, Ö Schäfer J, C Samson A, Naumann E, Tuschen-Caffier B. Emotional awareness in depressive and anxiety symptoms in youth: a meta-analytic review. J Youth Adolesc. Apr 2017;46(4):687-700. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Thompson RJ, Kuppens P, Mata J, Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, et al. Emotional clarity as a function of neuroticism and major depressive disorder. Emotion. Oct 2015;15(5):615-624. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Vine V, Aldao A. Impaired emotional clarity and psychopathology: a transdiagnostic deficit with symptom-specific pathways through emotion regulation. J Soc Clin Psychol. Apr 2014;33(4):319-342. [CrossRef]
- Park J, Naragon-Gainey K. Is more emotional clarity always better? An examination of curvilinear and moderated associations between emotional clarity and internalising symptoms. Cogn Emot. Mar 2020;34(2):273-287. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lischetzke T, Angelova R, Eid M. Validating an indirect measure of clarity of feelings: evidence from laboratory and naturalistic settings. Psychol Assess. Jun 2011;23(2):447-455. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Eckland NS, Thompson RJ. State emotional clarity is an indicator of fluid emotional intelligence ability. J Intell. Oct 10, 2023;11(10):196. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Salovey P, Mayer JD, Goldman SL, Turvey C, Palfai TP. Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Am Psychol Assoc. 1995:125-154. [CrossRef]
- Kaufman EA, Xia M, Fosco G, Yaptangco M, Skidmore CR, Crowell SE. The difficulties in emotion regulation scale short form (DERS-SF): validation and replication in adolescent and adult samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. Nov 23, 2015;38(3):443-455. [CrossRef]
- Lucas RE, Baird BM. Global self-assessment. In: Eid M, Diener E, editors. Handbook of Multimethod Measurement in Psychology. Washington, DC. American Psychological Association; 2006.
- Arndt C, Lischetzke T, Crayen C, Eid M. The assessment of emotional clarity via response times to emotion items: shedding light on the response process and its relation to emotion regulation strategies. Cogn Emot. May 2018;32(3):530-548. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Blöte AW, Westenberg PM. The temporal association between emotional clarity and depression symptoms in adolescents. J Adolesc. Feb 2019;71:110-118. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hernandez R, Hoogendoorn C, Gonzalez JS, Jin H, Pyatak EA, Spruijt-Metz D, et al. Reliability and validity of noncognitive ecological momentary assessment survey response times as an indicator of cognitive processing speed in people's natural environment: intensive longitudinal study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. May 30, 2023;11:e45203. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Schneider S, Hernandez R, Junghaenel DU, Orriens B, Lee PJ, Stone AA. Response times in Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA): shedding light on the response process with a drift diffusion model. Curr Psychol. May 27, 2023;43(7):5868-5886. [CrossRef]
- Ratcliff R, McKoon G. The diffusion decision model: theory and data for two-choice decision tasks. Neural Comput. Apr 2008;20(4):873-922. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Molenaar D, Tuerlinckx F, Maas HL. Fitting diffusion item response theory models for responses and response times using the R package diffIRT. J Stat Softw. 2015;66(4):1-34. [CrossRef]
- Ferrando PJ. Person-item distance and response time: an empirical study in personality measurement. Psicológica. Jan 2006;27(1):137-148. [FREE Full text]
- Medvedev ON, Roemer A, Krägeloh CU, Sandham MH, Siegert RJ. Enhancing the precision of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) using Rasch analysis. Curr Psychol. Mar 05, 2021;42(2):1554-1563. [CrossRef]
- Mascarenhas Fonseca L, Strong RW, Singh S, Bulger JD, Cleveland M, Grinspoon E, et al. Glycemic variability and fluctuations in cognitive status in adults with type 1 diabetes (GluCog): observational study using ecological momentary assessment of cognition. JMIR Diabetes. Jan 05, 2023;8:e39750. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kwasnicka D, Kale D, Schneider V, Keller J, Yeboah-Asiamah Asare B, Powell D, et al. Systematic review of ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies of five public health-related behaviours: review protocol. BMJ Open. Jul 16, 2021;11(7):e046435. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wrzus C, Neubauer AB. Ecological momentary assessment: a meta-analysis on designs, samples, and compliance across research fields. Assessment. Apr 2023;30(3):825-846. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Scott SB, Sliwinski MJ, Zawadzki M, Stawski RS, Kim J, Marcusson-Clavertz D, et al. A coordinated analysis of variance in affect in daily life. Assessment. Dec 2020;27(8):1683-1698. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Pyatak EA, Hernandez R, Pham LT, Mehdiyeva K, Schneider S, Peters A, et al. Function and emotion in everyday life with type 1 diabetes (FEEL-T1D): protocol for a fully remote intensive longitudinal study. JMIR Res Protoc. Oct 22, 2021;10(10):e30901. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Russell LB, Suh DC, Safford MA. Time requirements for diabetes self-management: too much for many? J Fam Pract. Jan 2005;54(1):52-56. [Medline]
- Gendelman N, Snell-Bergeon JK, McFann K, Kinney G, Paul Wadwa R, Bishop F, et al. Prevalence and correlates of depression in individuals with and without type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care. Apr 2009;32(4):575-579. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Coccaro EF, Lazarus S, Joseph J, Wyne K, Drossos T, Phillipson L, et al. Emotional regulation and diabetes distress in adults with type 1 and type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. Jan 2021;44(1):20-25. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kumar N, Unnikrishnan B, Thapar R, Mithra P, Kulkarni V, Holla R, et al. Distress and its effect on adherence to antidiabetic medications among type 2 diabetes patients in coastal South India. J Nat Sci Biol Med. 2017;8(2):216-220. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Halepian L, Saleh MB, Hallit S, Khabbaz LR. Adherence to insulin, emotional distress, and trust in physician among patients with diabetes: a cross-sectional study. Diabetes Ther. Apr 2018;9(2):713-726. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Aikens JE. Prospective associations between emotional distress and poor outcomes in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. Dec 2012;35(12):2472-2478. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Extremera N, Fernández-Berrocal P. Perceived emotional intelligence and life satisfaction: predictive and incremental validity using the Trait Meta-Mood Scale. Pers Individ Differ. Oct 2005;39(5):937-948. [CrossRef]
- Roque N, Sliwinski M, Ram N. Questionnaire-based everyday reaction time: reliable, valid, and unobtrusive measure of cognition. Innov Aging. Dec 16, 2020;4(Supplement_1):599. [CrossRef]
- Scott SB, Ram N, Smyth JM, Almeida DM, Sliwinski MJ. Age differences in negative emotional responses to daily stressors depend on time since event. Dev Psychol. Jan 2017;53(1):177-190. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS. The circumplex model of affect: an integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17(3):715-734. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- ilumivu homepage. ilumivu. URL: https://ilumivu.com/ [accessed 2024-07-02]
- Jaso BA, Kraus NI, Heller AS. Identification of careless responding in ecological momentary assessment research: from posthoc analyses to real-time data monitoring. Psychol Methods. Dec 2022;27(6):958-981. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Drift diffusion model on EMA items. OSF Home. Aug 1, 2022. URL: https://osf.io/r82hn/ [accessed 2024-07-02]
- Muthén LK, Muthén B. Mplus User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA. Muthén & Muthén; 2017.
- Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling Multidiscip J. 1999;6(1):1-55. [CrossRef]
- Geldhof GJ, Preacher KJ, Zyphur MJ. Reliability estimation in a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis framework. Psychol Methods. Mar 2014;19(1):72-91. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess. Feb 1985;49(1):71-75. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WBJ. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J Res Personal. Dec 2003;37(6):504-528. [CrossRef]
- Kroenke K, Strine TW, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Berry JT, Mokdad AH. The PHQ-8 as a measure of current depression in the general population. J Affect Disord. Apr 2009;114(1-3):163-173. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. May 22, 2006;166(10):1092-1097. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- McGuire BE, Morrison TG, Hermanns N, Skovlund S, Eldrup E, Gagliardino J, et al. Short-form measures of diabetes-related emotional distress: the Problem Areas in Diabetes Scale (PAID)-5 and PAID-1. Diabetologia. Jan 2010;53(1):66-69. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sliwinski MJ, Mogle JA, Hyun J, Munoz E, Smyth JM, Lipton RB. Reliability and validity of ambulatory cognitive assessments. Assessment. Jan 2018;25(1):14-30. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Salthouse TA. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol Psychol. Oct 2000;54(1-3):35-54. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Raykov T, Marcoulides GA. On multilevel model reliability estimation from the perspective of structural equation modeling. Struct Equ Modeling Multidiscip J. 2006;13(1):130-141. [CrossRef]
- Hallquist MN, Wiley JF. MplusAutomation: an R package for facilitating large-scale latent variable analyses in Mplus. Struct Equ Modeling. 2018;25(4):621-638. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2019. URL: https://www.R-project.org/ [accessed 2021-01-14]
- Hawks ZW, Strong R, Jung L, Beck ED, Passell EJ, Grinspoon E, et al. Accurate prediction of momentary cognition from intensive longitudinal data. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. Aug 2023;8(8):841-851. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Latent variable centering of predictors and mediators in multilevel and time-series models. Struct Equ Modeling Multidiscip J. Sep 28, 2018;26(1):119-142. [CrossRef]
- Examining the reliability and validity of ecological momentary assessment response time based measures of emotional clarity. OSF Home. Nov 9, 2023. URL: https://osf.io/gc7ez/ [accessed 2024-07-02]
- Leon AC. Multiplicity-adjusted sample size requirements: a strategy to maintain statistical power with Bonferroni adjustments. J Clin Psychiatry. Nov 2004;65(11):1511-1514. [Medline]
- Lizeretti NP, Extremera N. Emotional intelligence and clinical symptoms in outpatients with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). Psychiatr Q. Sep 2011;82(3):253-260. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Vine V, Marroquín B. Affect intensity moderates the association of emotional clarity with emotion regulation and depressive symptoms in unselected and treatment-seeking samples. Cognit Ther Res. Feb 2018;42(1):1-15. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Flynn M, Rudolph KD. A prospective examination of emotional clarity, stress responses, and depressive symptoms during early adolescence. J Early Adolesc. 2014;34(7):923-939. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Oropesa Ruiz NF, Mercader Rubio I, Gutiérrez Ángel N, Pérez García MA. Neuroticism and emotional intelligence in adolescence: a mediation model moderate by negative affect and self-esteem. Behav Sci (Basel). Jul 19, 2022;12(7):241. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Abeyta AA, Routledge C, Juhl J, Robinson MD. Finding meaning through emotional understanding: emotional clarity predicts meaning in life and adjustment to existential threat. Motiv Emot. Jun 4, 2015;39(6):973-983. [CrossRef]
- Ramos-Díaz E, Rodríguez-Fernández A, Axpe I, Ferrara M. Perceived emotional intelligence and life satisfaction among adolescent students: the mediating role of resilience. J Happiness Stud. Nov 24, 2018;20(8):2489-2506. [CrossRef]
- Althouse AD. Adjust for multiple comparisons? It's not that simple. Ann Thorac Surg. May 2016;101(5):1644-1645. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rothman KJ. Six persistent research misconceptions. J Gen Intern Med. Jul 2014;29(7):1060-1064. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rothman KJ. No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology. Jan 1990;1(1):43-46. [Medline]
- Veazie PJ. When to combine hypotheses and adjust for multiple tests. Health Serv Res. Jun 2006;41(3 Pt 1):804-818. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lotfaliany M, Bowe SJ, Kowal P, Orellana L, Berk M, Mohebbi M. Depression and chronic diseases: co-occurrence and communality of risk factors. J Affect Disord. Dec 01, 2018;241:461-468. [CrossRef] [Medline]
Abbreviations
CFI: comparative fit index |
DERS: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale |
DERS-SF: Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale short form |
EMA: ecological momentary assessment |
IRT: item response theory |
MSEM: multilevel structural equation model |
NA: negative affect |
PA: positive affect |
RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation |
RT: response time |
SRMR: standardized root mean square residual |
T1D: type 1 diabetes |
TLI: Tucker-Lewis Index |
Edited by J Torous; submitted 13.03.24; peer-reviewed by LMM Kivelae, MR Frumkin; comments to author 27.04.24; revised version received 17.05.24; accepted 05.06.24; published 18.07.24.
Copyright©Raymond Hernandez, Claire Hoogendoorn, Jeffrey S Gonzalez, Elizabeth A Pyatak, Gladys Crespo-Ramos, Stefan Schneider. Originally published in JMIR Mental Health (https://mental.jmir.org), 18.07.2024.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Mental Health, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://mental.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.