Review
Abstract
Background: There has been a surge in interest in examining internet gaming disorder (IGD) and its associations with gaming motivation. Three broad components of gaming motivation have been proposed: achievement, immersion, and social. Achievement-oriented players are motivated by gaining in-game rewards, immersion-oriented players are motivated by the experience of immersion in the virtual world, and social-oriented players are motivated by the need to socialize with other players through gaming.
Objective: This study aimed to (1) quantitatively synthesize the growing body of literature to systematically examine the discrepancies in the magnitude of associations between various components of gaming motivation and IGD and (2) examine the moderating role of cultural dimension on the association between escapism gaming motivation and IGD.
Methods: We conducted a systematic search of multiple databases between 2002 and 2020. Studies were included if they (1) included quantitative data, (2) used measures assessing both gaming motivation and IGD, and (3) contained sufficient information for effect size calculation.
Results: The findings revealed IGD to have a stronger association with achievement motivation (r=0.32) than with immersion (r=0.22) or social motivation (r=0.20), but the strongest such association was found to be with escapism motivation (r=0.40), a subcomponent of immersion motivation. Our cross-cultural comparison further showed a stronger association between escapism motivation and IGD in studies conducted in individualistic (vs collectivistic) regions.
Conclusions: This meta-analysis highlights the importance of acknowledging the discrepancies among different components of gaming motivation with respect to their role in the development of IGD, as well as the potential cultural variations in the strength of such associations.
doi:10.2196/23700
Keywords
Introduction
Background
Internet gaming is a popular leisure activity in the present digital age. However, gaming can become problematic when it interferes with a player’s psychological and social functioning [
, ]. Considering the prevalence of this emergent problem worldwide [ , ], problematic gaming was included as a “condition for further study” in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), under the label of “Internet Gaming Disorder” (IGD) [ ], and considerable research efforts have been made to conceptualize and assess IGD [ , ]. After IGD was listed as a “condition for further study” in the DSM-5 in 2013, gaming disorder was also officially included in the International Classification of Diseases, Eleventh Revision (ICD-11) in 2018. Thus far, only 1 measure has been developed based on the ICD-11 criteria, but none of the studies retrieved in this meta-analysis used this measure. This work thus focuses on IGD because several measures included in the present meta-analysis were constructed based on the DSM criteria.Some scholars conceptualize this disorder to be a form of behavioral addiction because some of its symptoms are similar to those of other behavioral addictions (eg, gambling disorder) and substance use disorder [
]. Based on the criteria for other behavioral addictions, several recurring symptoms of IGD have been identified, including preoccupation with gaming activities and withdrawal symptoms such as irritability when individuals are kept from playing [ , ]. Studies have revealed that IGD was positively associated with mental health issues such as depression, social anxiety, and psychological distress [ - ].Gaming Motivation as an Underlying Factor of IGD
As gaming is generally regarded as a healthy leisure activity, scholars have advocated for an examination of IGD from the viewpoint of etiological factors such as gaming motivation [
, ]. Although the specific gaming motivation differs considerably among players, the driving force behind IGD stems from players’ over-reliance on gaming to fulfill certain psychological needs [ ], a postulation corroborated by evidence on the positive associations between various types of gaming motivation and IGD [ , ]. Nevertheless, several issues remain unexplored, and we conducted a meta-analysis to quantitatively synthesize the available evidence to address 2 of these important but unresolved issues.The first issue is the empirical inconsistencies regarding the associations between gaming motivations and IGD. For instance, some studies indicate that certain types of gaming motivation, such as the desire to achieve in-game success, are more strongly associated with IGD than are other types of gaming motivation, such as the motivation to form social relations through gaming [
, ]. However, the evidence remains inconclusive because many studies in this area have examined only a single type of gaming motivation, thereby rendering between-motivation comparisons impossible [ ]. Second, cross-cultural comparisons are essential for studying IGD as a global phenomenon [ ]. For instance, although intervention programs for IGD are more commonly observed in Asian countries characterized by collectivistic orientations [ - ], the implications and the potential influence of such cultural differences on the association between gaming motivation and IGD has yet to be empirically investigated.Components of Gaming Motivation
To identify specific components of gaming motivation that play an influential role on IGD, we adopted Yee’s categorization [
] as our theoretical foundation for a systematic analysis. Among the various frameworks available in the literature, we chose Yee’s framework [ ] because it is the most comprehensive one that adopts a hierarchical structure encompassing both components and subcomponents and is also by far the most widely adopted framework in the literature of gaming motivation. This tridimensional framework categorizes gaming motivation into 3 broad components. First, achievement motivation comprises gaming motivations pertaining to the desire to achieve in-game recognition, power, and status. Second, social motivation refers to gaming motivations related to the urge to build connections and interact with other players. Third, immersion motivation includes those motivations pertaining to the desire to experience the virtual in-game world.The hypothesized discrepancies among the achievement, social, and immersion motivation components are corroborated by prior studies indicating that the 3 components have distinct magnitudes of associations with IGD. For instance, the achievement component has been consistently identified as a robust indicator of IGD, with a moderately positive association between the 2 constructs [
, ]. Specifically, scholars have postulated that players who lack success in real life often choose to compensate for such deficiencies with their achievements in the gaming world [ ]. Hence, players who overvalue their in-game achievements often need to endure extensive gameplay to achieve the occasional moments of success, which in turn increases their propensity to develop IGD [ ].Another body of studies has revealed weak to moderate positive associations between immersion motivation and IGD [
, ]. Immersion motivation entails the incentive to immerse oneself in gaming, as the highly immersive nature of internet games may serve as a refuge for players to escape from real-life difficulties (eg, social anxiety, loneliness) [ , ]. Although gaming may provide temporary relief from such difficulties, it can also create problematic beliefs about the maladaptive use of gaming to constantly avoid real-life issues [ ].Mixed findings, however, have been reported for the association between social motivation and IGD. Although some studies have documented weak to moderate positive associations between the 2 constructs [
, ], others have reported null or even contradictory findings [ , ]. Such inconsistencies may be related to the complex social outcomes of gaming. For instance, although players with a stronger social motivation are more likely to receive online social support through gaming [ , ], these online relations have also been linked to the social “obligation” of gaming [ ]. More specifically, players who want to become core members of a gaming team are often obligated to play for as long as other team members want to play. This perceived obligation of gaming often increases players’ gameplay time and gaming-related distress, both of which contribute to the onset of IGD [ , ].Studies examining all 3 motivation components have compared the associations of these components with IGD. For example, studies investigating both achievement and immersion motivations have revealed IGD to be more strongly associated with the former than the latter [
, ]. Moreover, IGD has also been consistently shown to have a weaker association with social motivation than with achievement motivation [ ] or immersion motivation [ ].Based on these empirical findings, we hypothesize that the 3 components of gaming motivation are differentially associated with IGD. More specifically, we predict that this meta-analysis may reveal the association between achievement motivation and IGD to be stronger than that between immersion motivation and IGD. Moreover, social motivation is expected to have a weaker association than achievement or immersion motivation with IGD.
Subcomponents of Gaming Motivation
In addition to the 3 motivation components conceptualized in the tridimensional gaming motivation framework, each motivation component also comprises multiple subcomponents. Moreover, prior studies have revealed sizable differences among specific subcomponents of the same motivation component with respect to their association with IGD [
, ]. To empirically evaluate these within-component variations, we adopted Yee’s 10-factor motivation taxonomy [ ] that includes 10 motivation subcomponents, each of which is classified under 1 of the 3 aforementioned gaming motivation components (ie, achievement, immersion, and social).According to the 10-factor taxonomy, immersion gaming motivation comprises 4 subcomponents: discovery, role-play, customization, and escapism. Discovery motivation refers to players’ interest in exploring the virtual in-game world, whereas role-play motivation reflects players’ incentive to create and identify with their in-game avatars. Customization motivation refers to players’ desire to alter various aspects of their in-game characters as they wish, whereas escapism motivation reflects their urge to use gaming activities to escape from real-life difficulties. Studies have revealed considerable differences in the association of each of the 4 subcomponents with IGD. For example, escapism motivation has been consistently documented to have a moderate to strong association with IGD [
], whereas the discovery and customization motivation subcomponents display a much weaker such association [ , ].These within-component variations have been attributed to the stronger conceptual connections between escapism motivation and IGD than between the other subcomponents and IGD [
]. According to Baumeister’s escape-from-self theory [ ], the assertion that immersion-oriented players use gaming to evade real-life adversities is more applicable to escapism motivation [ ], primarily because that subcomponent refers explicitly to such desire [ ]. In contrast, the other subcomponents, such as discovery and customization motivations, do not entail similar incentives to use gaming as a means to escape. It is thus important to disentangle the distinctions between escapism motivation and the other subcomponents of immersion motivation in this meta-analysis.In contrast to the substantial differences among the subcomponents of immersion motivation, there are fewer distinctions among the subcomponents of achievement motivation and social motivation with respect to their association with IGD. There are 3 subcomponents of achievement motivation. Advancement motivation refers to the desire to accumulate in-game rewards, mechanics motivation describes the urge to understand the gameplay mechanism, and competition motivation refers to the incentive to challenge and outcompete other players. Studies have shown moderately positive associations between all 3 subcomponents and IGD [
, ].Social motivation also comprises 3 subcomponents. Socializing motivation describes players’ intention to establish casual social relations with other players, relationship motivation refers to players’ desire to maintain long-term social relations through gaming, and teamwork motivation describes players’ interest in collaborating with other players during gameplay. Past studies have revealed few differences among the 3 social motivation subcomponents with respect to their associations with IGD [
, ].Considering these findings, we hypothesize that there will be significant differences among the immersion motivation subcomponents in terms of their associations with IGD, but there will be no such significant differences among the subcomponents of achievement motivation or those of social motivation.
Cultural Dimensions
We further posited that the magnitude of the association between gaming motivation and IGD varied by some cultural factors. Cross-cultural psychologists have maintained that people’s thoughts and behaviors are often shaped by the culture in which they reside [
, ]. Specifically, one of the most widely studied cultural dimensions is individualism-collectivism [ , ]. In individualistic cultures, the self-interest of individuals is generally regarded to be of greater importance than the collective interests of the group, whereas in collectivistic cultures, it is the collective interests of the group that take precedence [ , ].We hypothesize that the cultural dimension of individualism-collectivism may also explain the differential magnitudes of the association between IGD and gaming motivation, escapism motivation in particular. Such assertion is made based on past studies that documented cross-cultural differences in the way that members of a society interpret the non-normative behaviors such as excessive gaming. For instance, members of individualistic societies tend to view behavioral addiction or substance addiction as a consequence of one’s own choices or personality, whereas members of collectivistic societies tend to attribute it to such interpersonal factors as the influence of the social environment or such institutional factors as government policy [
].In the context of IGD, such cultural differences are reflected in the preventive approaches implemented in different regions. For example, legislation concerning prevention programs for IGD is more commonly observed in collectivistic countries such as Japan and South Korea [
]. These regions place stronger emphasis on the need for parental and teacher supervision of youngsters’ gaming activities [ , ]. Such policies are less common in Western individualistic countries. Hence, in societies emphasizing collectivistic values, players with stronger escapism motivation may more likely receive attention from their parents or teachers and benefit from prevention programs before their gaming activities evolve into problematic or addictive gaming. In individualistic societies, where similar prevention programs are less accessible, players who wish to escape real-life issues through gaming may not receive sufficient social support and attention to prevent the onset of IGD. In summary, we hypothesize that the association between escapism motivation and IGD may be stronger in studies conducted in individualistic (vs collectivistic) regions.Methods
Literature Search
The PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) guidelines were adhered to when this meta-analysis was performed and when the findings were reported. To identify relevant reports for this meta-analysis, we performed a multistage literature search using diverse methods in April 2019 with an update in March 2020. In the first stage, electronic bibliographic searches were conducted using 3 meta-databases: ProQuest, EBSCOhost, and Scopus. The following Boolean string was used in the search for relevant articles: (“Internet gam*” OR “online gam*” OR “digital gam*”) AND (“disorder” OR “abuse” OR “addict*” OR “compulsi*” OR “dependenc*” OR “excess*” OR “pathologic*” OR “problematic”).
In the next stage, we manually located reports that could not be identified in the first stage. First, the reference sections of previous meta-analyses related to the topic of IGD were scanned [
, ]. Second, bibliographic searches were performed on the aforementioned meta-databases for studies citing instruments of IGD and gaming motivation. Third, a review of the “gray literature” was performed to identify relevant unpublished reports (eg, regional literature databases, online archives). In the final stage, we contacted authors of some reports identified for further clarification and the provision of missing data.Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
To be included in this review, reports identified in the literature search had to contain at least one quantitative measure of IGD and at least one quantitative measure of gaming motivation. Reports were excluded if they (1) included qualitative data alone, (2) used measures assessing only the problematic use of internet activities or offline gaming but not internet gaming, (3) employed a measure of either IGD or gaming motivation but not both, or (4) lacked sufficient information for effect size calculation. No exclusions were made based on language.
Report Selection Process
Two independent reviewers performed the initial screening of the reports based on their titles, abstracts, keywords, or a combination thereof. After the initial screening, the full text of the remaining reports was retrieved for data extraction.
presents a PRISMA flow diagram illustrating the report selection process, which yielded the final pool of 49 eligible reports.Coding of Effect Sizes and Moderators
Data Extraction Process
The 2 reviewers who performed the screening task also independently extracted the following data: number of participants, sex and age composition, research design, geographical location of the study, measures of IGD and gaming motivation used, gaming motivation components and subcomponents, and the corresponding effect size estimates for the associations between IGD and gaming motivation. Any discrepancies in coding were resolved by a discussion with the senior author who was not involved in the coding process. The interrater reliability index of Krippendorff α coefficient was .92 [
], indicating a high level of interreviewer reliability.Effect Size Metric
The Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient (r) was selected as the effect size metric for its ease of interpretation. When such an effect size could not be found, other relevant statistics (ie, chi-square, Cohen d) were extracted and then converted into r using the “compute.es” statistical package in the R programing environment (version 4.0.0; R Foundation for Statistical Computing) [
, ].Coding of Effect Size Estimates
The reviewers extracted all relevant effect size estimates pertaining to the associations between gaming motivation and IGD. We extracted all effect size estimates of the associations of IGD with the 3 gaming motivation components (ie, achievement, immersion, and social) and IGD and with the gaming motivation subcomponents and IGD based on the aforementioned 10-factor motivation model [
]. For the studies that used instruments based on this model or other conceptually similar models, effect size estimates were extracted.A Priori Moderators
All the a priori moderators described in the Introduction were coded during data extraction. First, the motivation component was coded as a 3-level categorical variable (ie, achievement, immersion, and social). Second, the subcomponents categorized under each motivation component were coded as a categorical variable. For instance, the subcomponents related to achievement motivation were coded as advancement, mechanics, and competition, and those related to social motivation were coded as socializing, relationship, and teamwork. Those related to immersion motivation were coded as a 4-level categorical variable: discovery motivation, role-playing motivation, customization motivation, and escapism motivation.
Finally, the moderator variable of individualism was defined as a continuous variable, with higher scores assigned to regions with higher individualistic cultural values. We first recorded the geographical locations in which individual samples were recruited and then ascribed a national individualism score to each sample based on Hofstede’s data matrix [
].Meta-analytic Procedures
Three-level mixed-effects meta-analysis was adopted in this study. Conventional meta-analytic methods may generate biased effect size estimates when effect sizes are not independent. In this meta-analysis, there were many reports (77%) with multiple effect sizes for the associations between the various components of gaming motivation and IGD. It was thus necessary to perform 3-level meta-analysis using the structure of a multilevel model, with level 1 analyzing effect sizes, level 2 analyzing independent samples, and level 3 analyzing individual reports [
].Main effect analysis was conducted using the metaSEM package version 1.2.0 [
] and metafor package version 2.4.0 [ ] in the R programming environment [ ]. Such analyses examined the magnitude and direction of the association of IGD with the various gaming motivation components and subcomponents. Maximum likelihood estimation was performed to estimate 95% CI.The Cochran Q statistic was examined to evaluate the presence of heterogeneity and determine whether the effect size was consistent across reports. In addition, the I2 index was also calculated to evaluate between-study heterogeneity. If both the Q statistic and I2 index indicated a significant level of heterogeneity, 3-level mixed-effects meta-analysis was performed to further examine the variability across reports using the metaSEM package.
Detection of Possible Bias Risks (Omnibus Analysis)
Study Quality
The quality of each report was evaluated based on 8 indicators [
, ]: sampling method, sample heterogeneity, statistical power, sample description, measurement validity, measurement reliability, study methodology, and study design. The criteria used for coding the various indicators are summarized in . An individual report was assigned a value of 1 for meeting the criteria of each indicator and a value of 0 for failing to do so, except for scale reliability and scale validity, both of which were coded as continuous variables. To determine whether the quality of individual reports exerted an influence on the magnitude of a correlation, we tested the moderating effects of study quality using the 8 individual indicators and the composite score.Quality indicatorsa | Criteria met (score=1) | Criteria not met (score=0) |
Sampling method | Sampling involved random selection of participations | Sampling involved nonrandom methods |
Sample heterogeneity | Sample comprised 2 or more demographic groups | Sample included only a single group |
Statistical power | Sample size was large enough to yield adequate statistical power | Sample size was not sufficient to yield adequate statistic power |
Sample description | Demographic information was clearly described | Demographic information was not clearly described |
Measurement validity | All scales used in the study were validated | None were validated |
Measurement reliability | All scales were reliable (Cronbach α≥.70) | No scales were reliable (Cronbach α≥.70) |
Study methodology | Study adopted 2 or more methodologies | Study adopted only 1 methodology |
Study design | Data were collected at 2 or more time points | Data were collected at only 1 time point |
aThe indicators of scale reliability and scale validity were coded as continuous variables (ie, percentage of scales that were reliable or valid, respectively). The other indicators were all coded as dichotomous variables.
Publication Bias
To address the potential issues of publication bias, we adopted several common methods. First, the fail-safe N was employed to estimate the number of unpublished or missing reports with an effect size averaging 0 that would nullify the effect observed in the meta-analysis. Publication bias was a potential concern if the fail-safe N was smaller than 5k + 10 (k = the number of studies) based on established criteria [
]. Second, publication bias was evaluated by funnel plots and Egger tests derived from weighted regression [ ], with asymmetrical funnel plots indicating publication bias. The potential issue of asymmetry was also indicated by significant Egger test results. Finally, we employed the trim-and-fill technique, which estimates the number of missing reports and provides an estimate of the effect size after publication bias has been taken into account [ ]. If the adjusted effect size is vastly different from that yielded prior to such adjustment, publication bias is a likely concern. These analyses were performed using the metafor package in the R programming environment.Results
Descriptions of the Data Set
The majority of the 49 studies included in this analysis were published in academic journals (89%), and the remainder included 6 unpublished dissertations and 2 conference proceedings. Most were published after 2008, when studies on IGD began to be seen. Moreover, 74% were published after 2014, when the American Psychiatric Association listed IGD in the DSM-5 as a condition for further study [
].The aggregate number of participants in the meta-analysis of this study was 52,254 (mean 1022, SD 1249.37, range 59-5222). The participants were recruited from 86 independent samples. The mean age across the samples was 26.51 (SD 5.03, range 12.90-34.23), and 68% of the participants were men. The samples were recruited from 23 countries across multiple geographical regions, including Europe (54%), Asia (25%), North America (16%), and other regions (5%).
To assess IGD symptoms, some of the included studies adopted the DSM criteria [
]. Other popular such measures included the Addiction-Engagement Questionnaire [ ], Game Addiction Scale [ ], Internet Gaming Disorder Test [ ], and Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire [ ]. Moreover, some studies adapted Young’s Internet Addition Test [ ] to measure IGD symptoms. To assess gaming motivation, most of the included studies employed the Motivation to Play in Online Games Questionnaire [ ], followed by the Motives for Online Gaming Questionnaire [ ] and Online Gaming Motivations Scale [ ]. All the subscales of these gaming motivation measures can be mapped onto Yee’s hierarchical model [ ], except for the “fantasy” subscale. The results involving this subscale were omitted in this meta-analysis.Main-Effect Meta-analysis
Gaming Motivation Components and IGD
To examine the associations between the 3 gaming motivation components (ie, achievement, social, and immersion) and IGD, 3 sets of 3-level meta-analysis were performed based on 64 effect size estimates extracted from 22 reports. The results are summarized in
. Significant positive correlations were identified for the association between each gaming motivation component and IGD. The correlation between the achievement motivation and IGD was 0.32, indicating a medium effect size, whereas that between the other 2 types of motivation and IGD was small to medium in magnitude (r=0.20 and r=0.22).As shown in
, the Cochran’s Q test results revealed significant heterogeneity in the effect size estimates of all 3 gaming motivation components. In addition, the I2 statistics indicated a moderate to high degree of between-study heterogeneity [ ]. Accordingly, 3-level mixed-effects meta-analysis was performed to examine the hypothesized moderating effects of the 4 a priori moderators.Gaming motivation | ka value | rb value | 95% CI value | Qc value | I2d value | I2e value |
Achievement componentf | 21 | 0.32g1 | 0.27 to 0.36 | 182.21g | 0.91 | 0.03 |
Advancement subcomponent | 10 | 0.29g* | 0.25 to 0.35 | 310.14g | 0.52 | N/Ah |
Mechanics subcomponent | 9 | 0.28g* | 0.25 to 0.33 | 11.01 | 0.36 | N/A |
Competition subcomponent | 24 | 0.31g* | 0.23 to 0.42 | 1341.25g | 0.74 | 0.11 |
Social componenti | 27 | 0.20g2 | 0.14 to 0.29 | 844.90g | 0.78 | 0.18 |
Socializing subcomponent | 27 | 0.17g# | 0.09 to 0.26 | 1291.76g | 0.84 | 0.12 |
Relationship subcomponent | 11 | 0.25g* | 0.13 to 0.32 | 101.42g | 0.88 | N/A |
Teamwork subcomponent | 8 | 0.08$ | −0.09 to 0.22 | 145.01g | 0.49 | N/A |
Immersion componentj | 16 | 0.22g2 | 0.13 to 0.30 | 341.22g | 0.80 | 0.13 |
Discovery subcomponent | 8 | 0.07$ | −0.01 to 0.17 | 40.87g | 0.41 | N/A |
Role-play subcomponent | 24 | 0.22g# | 0.14 to 0.32 | 2143.22g | 0.54 | N/A |
Customization subcomponent | 8 | 0.21g# | 0.15 to 0.30 | 50.41g | 0.41 | N/A |
Escapism subcomponent | 44 | 0.40g* | 0.35 to 0.45 | 1510.22g | 0.70 | 0.21 |
ak: number of tested correlations.
br: pooled correlation coefficient.
cQ: Cochrane heterogeneity statistic.
dI2: level 2 heterogeneity index.
eI2: level 3 heterogeneity index.
fComprises the advancement, mechanics, and competition subcomponents.
gP<.001.
hN/A: not applicable.
iComprises the socializing, relationship, and teamwork subcomponents.
jComprises the discovery, role-play, customization, and escapism subcomponents.
Gaming Motivation Subcomponents and IGD
As summarized in
, significant positive correlations were identified for 8 of the subcomponents (r ranging from 0.17 to 0.40), but those for the teamwork and the discovery subcomponents were not significant (r=0.08 and r=0.07, respectively).The magnitude of the correlations between all 3 subcomponents of achievement motivation and IGD was moderate (r= 0.29 for advancement, r=0.28 for mechanics, and r=0.31 for competition). For the subcomponents of social motivation, the magnitude of the correlations with IGD was small to moderate for socializing and relationship motivation (r=0.17 and r=0.25, respectively) but not significant for teamwork motivation (r=0.08). The results for the subcomponents of immersion motivation were more diverse. The association between escapism motivation and IGD showed the strongest correlation (r=0.40), whereas the magnitude of the correlations with IGD was small to moderate for the role-play and the customization subcomponents (r=0.22 and r=0.21, respectively). The discovery motivation subcomponent was found to have no significant association with IGD (r=0.07).
The Cochran Q heterogeneity statistics indicated significant between-study heterogeneity for all the gaming motivation subcomponents except mechanics motivation (Q=20.14, P=.08). Similarly, the I2 statistics also revealed a moderate to high degree of between-study heterogeneity in the effect size estimates [
].Moderator Analysis
Moderating Role of Gaming Motivation Components
For the moderator of the gaming motivation component, the findings from the 3-level mixed-effects meta-analysis supported our hypothesis that this variable moderated the association between gaming motivation and IGD (QM=1199.27, P<.001). Pairwise comparisons indicated IGD to have a significantly stronger correlation with achievement motivation than with social or immersion motivation (all P<.001). However, only a marginally significant difference between the social motivation–IGD correlation and the immersion motivation–IGD correlation was identified (P=.15).
Moderating Role of Gaming Motivation Subcomponents
A series of 3-level mixed-effects meta-analysis was performed to examine the moderating effects of the gaming motivation subcomponents of the 3 motivation components.
In the first set, the results corroborated our hypothesis that the subcomponents of achievement motivation were not a significant moderator (QM=9.81, P=.09). In the second set, however, the results were contrary to our hypothesis concerning the subcomponents of social motivation, indicating that they significantly moderated the association of social motivation with IGD (QM=45.94, P=.01). Pairwise comparisons indicated significantly stronger correlations of IGD with socializing motivation than with relationship or teamwork motivation (all P<.01), and significant differences were identified between the relationship motivation–IGD correlation and the teamwork motivation–IGD correlation (P<.001).
In the third set, the results supported the hypothesized moderating effect of the subcomponents of immersion motivation (QM=602.84, P<.001). Further analysis based on pairwise comparisons revealed the correlation of IGD with escapism motivation to be significantly stronger than that with discovery, role-play, or customization motivations (all P<.001). Although no significant differences were identified between the role-play motivation–IGD correlation and the customization motivation–IGD correlation (P=.23), the correlation of IGD with the role-play and customization motivation subcomponents was significantly stronger than that with the discovery motivation subcomponent (P<.001).
Moderating Role of Individualism (vs Collectivism)
Three-level mixed-effects meta-analysis was also conducted to examine the hypothesized moderating role of individualism. The results supported our hypothesis that the cultural dimension of individualism had a significant moderating effect (b=−.0023, SE .0015; P<.001) on the correlation between the escapism subcomponent of immersion motivation and IGD. Simple slope analysis further revealed the positive correlation between IGD and escapism motivation to be stronger among the samples recruited from regions higher versus lower in individualism (r=0.47 vs r=0.25).
Detection of Possible Bias
Study Quality
Moderator analysis was performed to examine the potential influence of study quality on the meta-analysis results based on 8 indicators. The nonsignificant results revealed that study quality did not exert a significant influence on the association between any of the components or subcomponents of gaming motivation and IGD (all P ranging from .16 to .47).
Publication Bias
The various analyses similarly revealed no evidence of publication bias. First, the fail-safe N estimations computed based on the revisited file-drawer test (N ranging from 583 to 12,770) all exceeded the suggested cutoff value (5k + 10 = 255). Moreover, the trim-and-fill technique showed no evidence to suggest that publication bias would substantially alter the effect size estimates after the corresponding adjustments. Finally, the Egger regression test revealed no significant asymmetry (all P ranging from .09 to .68), indicating that the effect size estimates were unrelated to sample size.
Discussion
Summary of Findings
This meta-analysis synthesized the growing body of literature and addressed several unresolved research issues in the study of gaming motivation and IGD. First, the findings partially support the hypothesized discrepancies among the 3 gaming motivation components (ie, achievement, social, and immersion) with regard to their associations with IGD. Specifically, IGD tends to have a stronger association with achievement motivation than with immersion motivation or social motivation. However, there is no significant difference between immersion motivation and social motivation, with both showing a weak to moderate positive correlation with IGD. Hence, social motivation should also be acknowledged in the treatment of this emergent disorder.
Regarding the within-component variations, there are no significant differences among the subcomponents of achievement with regard to their associations with IGD. The results for the immersion motivation component are consistent with the escape-from-self theory [
], with the escapism motivation subcomponent demonstrating a considerably stronger association than the other subcomponents (ie, discovery, customization, and role-play motivations) with IGD. Moreover, interest in experiencing the virtual world (ie, discovery motivation) is not identified as a risk factor for IGD. This meta-analysis thus highlights the importance of distinguishing between motivation to explore the virtual world and motivation to use gaming to escape from various real-life issues.The results for the social motivation component contradict our hypothesis. Our findings show that the relationship subcomponent has a considerably stronger association than the socializing or the teamwork subcomponent with IGD. These findings substantiate the postulation that “social obligation” can contribute to excessive gameplay in socially oriented players [
]. Although the collaborative gameplay design of many online games may increase players’ online social capital [ , ], it can require players to unwillingly engage in gaming for the collective benefit of the gaming team [ ]. Such obligation can lead to excessive gameplay and decrease the resources needed to maintain offline social relations [ , ]. Moreover, the nonsignificant association between the teamwork subcomponent and IGD further indicates that collaborative gameplay itself does not directly influence the onset of IGD. Thus, only the socializing and the relationship motivation subcomponents should be acknowledged as relevant to IGD and its treatment.Implications for Assessment and Treatment
This meta-analysis has implications for researchers. Specifically, the considerable discrepancies identified among the subcomponents of social motivation and immersion motivation highlight the importance of assessing gaming motivation at the subcomponent level. The 3 broad components of gaming describe 3 distinct domains of motivation for gameplay, but these domains also encompass multiple subcomponents that need to be evaluated in diverse ways. For instance, exclusive examination of the social motivation component may lead to neglect of the differences between players’ interest in forming social relations and their interest in collaborating with other players. To provide a more comprehensive examination of gaming motivation, future studies should investigate gaming motivation at both the component and subcomponent levels.
Our meta-analysis also has implications for clinicians and practitioners. Some scholars have maintained that a shortcoming of current cognitive therapy–based interventions is the lack of measures to evaluate the cognitions associated with IGD [
, ]. To bridge this knowledge gap, discerning among the multiple motivations for gaming may help practitioners identify the underlying problematic cognition and use specific treatment programs. For example, clients with a stronger social gaming motivation may more likely believe gaming to be the only means of forming social relations; thus, it may be helpful to incorporate programs that strengthen their social skills in face-to-face interactions [ , ]. However, reviews of IGD treatment programs indicate that gaming motivation is rarely considered [ ]. Thus, practitioners should consider evaluating their clients’ gaming motivation and then tailor treatment programs to specifically address the corresponding problems.Research Caveats and Directions for Future Research
As our findings are limited by the available studies, there are several research caveats. First, it is noteworthy that Yee’s motivation taxonomy [
] was originally developed from samples of massively multiplayer online role-playing game players, so some of its motives (eg, role-play, customization) are specifically related to this game genre. Some studies have revealed certain genres (eg, massively multiplayer online role-playing game, first-person shooter) to have a stronger tendency to elicit IGD among players with specific types of gaming motivation, including social and immersion motivation [ ]. In response to the increasingly diversified gameplay designs, some scholars have called for greater effort in investigating the role of game genre in the motivation and behavior across players of various game genres [ , ]. Moreover, although most subscales of the included gaming motivation measures are covered in Yee’s framework [ ], there are a few exceptions (eg, fantasy) that cannot be mapped onto the framework. Researchers may integrate Yee’s framework with other existing ones to broaden coverage.Second, a large number of studies included in our meta-analysis recruited players who are highly committed to gaming, so our findings are not necessarily generalizable to the increasing number of casual players who play occasionally, as these players may have distinct interpretations of gaming motivations [
]. For example, committed players tend to perceive social gaming motivation as their desire to build and maintain online social relations through gaming activities [ ]. For casual players, however, this type of gaming motivation primarily indicates their interest in playing with members of their offline social circle in a group context [ ]. Thus, the scope of future studies should be expanded to include different types of players (eg, committed vs casual players) to allow comparisons of their potentially distinct interpretations of gaming motivation.Third, as our meta-analysis mainly included studies that examined IGD based on composite scores for multiple symptoms, our findings may not be generalizable to specific symptoms of IGD [
, ] such as preoccupation with gaming activities or unsuccessful attempts to control gaming activities. Studies using composite scores for multiple symptoms may be unable to distinguish among the varying magnitudes of the association of gaming motivation with different symptoms of IGD. For example, the associations between social gaming motivation and preoccupation with gaming activities and a deterioration in interpersonal relations were found to be stronger than the association between such motivation and academic problems [ ]. Future studies should thus investigate the specific symptoms of IGD to allow comparisons of their potential differing associations with gaming motivation.Finally, all the studies included in this systematic review adopted self-report questionnaires to assess both gaming motivation and IGD symptoms. The survey method is susceptible to various methodological biases in responding and recall [
], and no causal links can be inferred. Greater effort should be expended to the design of experiments or quasi-experiments to test the causal relationships between gaming motivation and IGD.Conclusions
This meta-analysis explores several issues in the study of gaming motivation and IGD. First, IGD was found to be more strongly associated with achievement motivation than with immersion motivation or social motivation; however, both of the latter components had weak positive associations with IGD. Second, our analysis at the subcomponent level strongly corroborates escape-from-self theory, with escapism motivation demonstrating a significantly stronger association than the other conceptually related subcomponents with IGD. Finally, our cross-cultural analysis identified the individualism-collectivism dimension to be a significant moderator of the association between escapism motivation and IGD, with a stronger such association found for studies conducted in individualistic countries than in those conducted in collectivistic countries.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this research was provided by the Hong Kong Research Grants Council’s General Research Fund (17400714) and the University of Hong Kong’s Seed Fund for Basic Research (201711159216).
Conflicts of Interest
None declared.
References
- Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf H, Mößle T, et al. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction 2014 Sep 23;109(9):1399-1406. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Tsui YY, Cheng C. Internet gaming disorder, risky online behaviour, and mental health in Hong Kong adolescents: the beneficial role of psychological resilience. Front Psychiatry 2021 Oct 15;12:722353 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Fam JY. Prevalence of internet gaming disorder in adolescents: a meta-analysis across three decades. Scand J Psychol 2018 Oct 13;59(5):524-531. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wang H, Cheng C. New perspectives on the prevalence and associated factors of gaming disorder in Hong Kong community adults: A generational approach. Comput Human Behav 2021 Jan;114:106574. [CrossRef]
- Wang H, Cheng C. Psychometric evaluation and comparison of two gaming disorder measures derived from the DSM-5 and ICD-11 frameworks. Front Psychiatry 2020 Dec 17;11:577366 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Cheng C, Cheung MW, Wang H. Multinational comparison of internet gaming disorder and psychosocial problems versus well-being: Meta-analysis of 20 countries. Comput Human Behav 2018 Nov;88:153-167. [CrossRef]
- Pontes HM, Király O, Demetrovics Z, Griffiths MD. The conceptualisation and measurement of DSM-5 Internet Gaming Disorder: the development of the IGD-20 Test. PLoS One 2014 Oct 14;9(10):e110137 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Petry NM, Rehbein F, Ko C, O'Brien CP. Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2015 Sep 29;17(9):72. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kaptsis D, King DL, Delfabbro PH, Gradisar M. Withdrawal symptoms in internet gaming disorder: A systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2016 Feb;43:58-66. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sigerson L, Li AY, Cheung MW, Cheng C. Examining common information technology addictions and their relationships with non-technology-related addictions. Comput Human Behav 2017 Oct;75:520-526. [CrossRef]
- Casale S, Musicò A, Schimmenti A. Beyond internalizing and externalizing symptoms: The association between body disconnection and the symptoms of Internet gaming disorder. Addict Behav 2021 Dec;123:107043. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kowal M, Conroy E, Ramsbottom N, Smithies T, Toth A, Campbell M. Gaming your mental health: a narrative review on mitigating symptoms of depression and anxiety using commercial video games. JMIR Serious Games 2021 Jun 16;9(2):e26575 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ohayon MM, Roberts L. Internet gaming disorder and comorbidities among campus-dwelling U.S. university students. Psychiatry Res 2021 Aug;302:114043. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Wang H, Sigerson L, Cheng C. Digital nativity and information technology addiction: age cohort versus individual difference approaches. Comput Human Behav 2019 Jan;90:1-9. [CrossRef]
- Zhang J, Zhou H, Geng F, Song X, Hu Y. Internet gaming disorder increases mind-wandering in young adults. Front Psychol 2020 Jan 28;11:619072 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sigerson L, Li AY, Cheung MW, Luk JW, Cheng C. Psychometric properties of the Chinese Internet Gaming Disorder Scale. Addict Behav 2017 Nov;74:20-26 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Király O, Nagygyörgy K, Griffiths M, Demetrovics Z. Problematic online gaming. In: Rosenberg KP, Curtiss FL, editors. Behavioral Addictions: Criteria, Evidence, and Treatment. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press; 2014:61-97.
- Mills DJ, Milyavskaya M, Heath NL, Derevensky JL. Gaming motivation and problematic video gaming: the role of needs frustration. Eur J Soc Psychol 2017 Dec 18;48(4):551-559. [CrossRef]
- King DL, Delfabbro PH. The cognitive psychology of Internet gaming disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 2014 Jun;34(4):298-308 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Ballabio M, Griffiths MD, Urbán R, Quartiroli A, Demetrovics Z, Király O. Do gaming motives mediate between psychiatric symptoms and problematic gaming? An empirical survey study. Addict Res Theory 2017 Mar 27;25(5):397-408. [CrossRef]
- Laconi S, Pirès S, Chabrol H. Internet gaming disorder, motives, game genres and psychopathology. Comput Human Behav 2017 Oct;75:652-659. [CrossRef]
- Zanetta Dauriat F, Zermatten A, Billieux J, Thorens G, Bondolfi G, Zullino D, et al. Motivations to play specifically predict excessive involvement in massively multiplayer online role-playing games: evidence from an online survey. Eur Addict Res 2011;17(4):185-189. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kwok N, Khoo A. Gamers' motivations and problematic gaming: An exploratory study of gamers in World of Warcraft. Int J Cyber Behav 2011;1(3):34-49. [CrossRef]
- Király O, Bőthe B, Ramos-Diaz J, Rahimi-Movaghar A, Lukavska K, Hrabec O, et al. Ten-Item Internet Gaming Disorder Test (IGDT-10): Measurement invariance and cross-cultural validation across seven language-based samples. Psychol Addict Behav 2019 Feb;33(1):91-103. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Chau C, Tsui YY, Cheng C. Gamification for internet gaming disorder prevention: evaluation of a Wise IT-Use (WIT) program for Hong Kong primary students. Front Psychol 2019 Nov 1;10:2468 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- King DL, Herd MC, Delfabbro PH. Motivational components of tolerance in Internet gaming disorder. Comput Human Behav 2018 Jan;78:133-141. [CrossRef]
- Li AY, Chau C, Cheng C. Development and validation of a parent-based program for preventing gaming disorder: the game over intervention. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019 Jun 04;16(11):1984 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Yee N. Motivations for play in online games. Cyberpsychol Behav 2006 Dec;9(6):772-775. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Beard CL, Wickham RE. Gaming-contingent self-worth, gaming motivation, and Internet Gaming Disorder. Comput Human Behav 2016 Aug;61:507-515. [CrossRef]
- Snodgrass JG, Dengah HJF, Lacy MG, Fagan J. A formal anthropological view of motivation models of problematic MMO play: achievement, social, and immersion factors in the context of culture. Transcult Psychiatry 2013 Apr 20;50(2):235-262. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Khan A, Muqtadir R. Motives of problematic and nonproblematic online gaming among adolescents and young adults. Pak J Psychol Res 2016:119-138.
- Hagström D, Kaldo V. Escapism among players of MMORPGs--conceptual clarification, its relation to mental health factors, and development of a new measure. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2014 Jan;17(1):19-25. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Cheng C, Lau Y, Luk JW. Social capital-accrual, escape-from-self, and time-displacement effects of internet use during the COVID-19 stay-at-home period: prospective, quantitative survey study. J Med Internet Res 2020 Dec 24;22(12):e22740 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kardefelt-Winther D. Problematizing excessive online gaming and its psychological predictors. Comput Human Behav 2014 Feb;31:118-122. [CrossRef]
- Chen C, Leung L. Are you addicted to Candy Crush Saga? An exploratory study linking psychological factors to mobile social game addiction. Telemat Inform 2016 Nov;33(4):1155-1166. [CrossRef]
- Kaye LK, Kowert R, Quinn S. The role of social identity and online social capital on psychosocial outcomes in MMO players. Comput Human Behav 2017 Sep;74:215-223. [CrossRef]
- Zhang F, Kaufman D. The impacts of social interactions in MMORPGs on older adults’ social capital. Comput Human Behav 2015 Oct;51:495-503. [CrossRef]
- Hilgard J, Engelhardt CR, Bartholow BD. Individual differences in motives, preferences, and pathology in video games: the gaming attitudes, motives, and experiences scales (GAMES). Front Psychol 2013;4:608 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Snodgrass JG, Lacy MG, Francois Dengah HJ, Fagan J, Most DE. Magical flight and monstrous stress: technologies of absorption and mental wellness in Azeroth. Cult Med Psychiatry 2011 Mar;35(1):26-62. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Zhong Z, Yao MZ. Gaming motivations, avatar-self identification and symptoms of online game addiction. Asian J Commun 2012 Dec 14;23(5):555-573. [CrossRef]
- Billieux J, Van der Linden M, Achab S, Khazaal Y, Paraskevopoulos L, Zullino D, et al. Why do you play World of Warcraft? An in-depth exploration of self-reported motivations to play online and in-game behaviours in the virtual world of Azeroth. Comput Human Behav 2013 Jan;29(1):103-109. [CrossRef]
- Deleuze J, Long J, Liu T, Maurage P, Billieux J. Passion or addiction? Correlates of healthy versus problematic use of videogames in a sample of French-speaking regular players. Addict Behav 2018 Jul;82:114-121. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Yee N. The demographics, motivations, and derived experiences of users of massively multi-user online graphical environments. Presence (Camb) 2006 Jun 01;15(3):309-329. [CrossRef]
- Deleuze J, Rochat L, Romo L, Van der Linden M, Achab S, Thorens G, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of addictive behaviors in a community sample: A latent class analysis. Addict Behav Rep 2015 Jun;1:49-56 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hussain Z, Williams GA, Griffiths MD. An exploratory study of the association between online gaming addiction and enjoyment motivations for playing massively multiplayer online role-playing games. Comput Human Behav 2015 Sep;50:221-230. [CrossRef]
- Baumeister R. Escaping the self: Alcoholism, spirituality, masochism, and other flights from the burden of selfhood. New York, NY: Basic Books; 1991.
- Kuss D. Internet gaming addiction: current perspectives. Psychol Res Behav Manag 2013;6:125-137 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kwon J, Chung C, Lee J. The effects of escape from self and interpersonal relationship on the pathological use of Internet games. Community Ment Health J 2011 Feb 23;47(1):113-121. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Internet gaming addiction: a systematic review of empirical research. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2011 Mar 16;10(2):278-296. [CrossRef]
- Cross N. The relationship of online gaming addiction with motivations to play and craving. OhioLINK ETD. Bowling Green, OH: Bowling Green State University; 2016. URL: https://etd.ohiolink.edu/apexprod/rws_etd/send_file/send?accession=bgsu1435084396&disposition=inline [accessed 2022-02-14]
- Lewis M. Massively multiplayer online roleplaying gaming: motivation to play, player typologies, and addiction. OhioLINK ETD. Columbus, OH: Ohio State University; 2016. URL: https://etd.ohiolink.edu/apexprod/rws_olink/r/1501/10?p10_etd_subid=114307&clear=10 [accessed 2022-02-14]
- Berry J, Poortinga Y, Segall M, Dasen P. Cross-cultural Psychology: Research and Applications. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2002:0521646170.
- Cheng C, Cheung MW, Montasem A, 44 members of the International Network of Well-Being Studies. Explaining differences in subjective well-being across 33 nations using multilevel models: universal personality, cultural relativity, and national income. J Pers 2016 Feb 12;84(1):46-58. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Hamamura T, Bettache K, Xu Y. Individualism and collectivism. In: Zeigler-Hill V, Shackelford TK, editors. Sage Handbook of Personality and Individual Differences. New York, NY: Sage; 2018:365-382.
- Schimmack U, Oishi S, Diener E. Individualism: a valid and important dimension of cultural differences between nations. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2005;9(1):17-31. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Cheng C, Jose PE, Sheldon KM, Singelis TM, Cheung MWL, Tiliouine H, Chun WY, et al. Sociocultural differences in self-construal and subjective well-being: a test of four cultural models. J Cross Cult Psychol 2010 Sep 09;42(5):832-855. [CrossRef]
- Triandis HC. Individualism-collectivism and personality. J Pers 2001 Dec;69(6):907-924. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Pöysti V, Majamäki M. Cultural understandings of the pathways leading to problem gambling: Medical disorder or failure of self-regulation? Addict Res Theory 2012 Jun 25;21(1):70-82. [CrossRef]
- King DL, Delfabbro PH, Doh YY, Wu AMS, Kuss DJ, Pallesen S, et al. Policy and prevention approaches for disordered and hazardous gaming and internet use: an international perspective. Prev Sci 2018 Feb;19(2):233-249. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Li AY, Lo BC, Cheng C. It is the family context that matters: concurrent and predictive effects of aspects of parent-child interaction on video gaming-related problems. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2018 Jun;21(6):374-380. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lee S. Parental restrictive mediation of children’s internet use: effective for what and for whom? New Media Soc 2012 Jul 13;15(4):466-481. [CrossRef]
- Hayes AF, Krippendorff K. Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Commun Methods Meas 2007 Apr;1(1):77-89. [CrossRef]
- Del Re AC. compute.es: Compute Effect Sizes. CRAN - Package compute.es. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/compute.es/index.html [accessed 2019-07-19]
- R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing Computer software. R: The R Project for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing URL: https://www.r-project.org/ [accessed 2019-07-19]
- Hofstede G. Research and VSM: Dimension data matrix. GEERT HOFSTEDE. 2018. URL: http://www.geerthofstede.com/research--vsm [accessed 2019-06-18]
- Cheung M. Meta-analysis: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach. West Sussex, UK: Wiley; Apr 10, 2015.
- Cheung MW. metaSEM: an R package for meta-analysis using structural equation modeling. Front Psychol 2014 Jan 05;5:1521 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-Analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Soft 2010;36(3):1-48. [CrossRef]
- Cheng C, Cheung MW, Lo BCY. Relationship of health locus of control with specific health behaviours and global health appraisal: a meta-analysis and effects of moderators. Health Psychol Rev 2016 Dec 12;10(4):460-477 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Cheng C, Wang H, Sigerson L, Chau C. Do the socially rich get richer? A nuanced perspective on social network site use and online social capital accrual. Psychol Bull 2019 Jul;145(7):734-764. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Rosenberg MS. The file-drawer problem revisited: a general weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis. Evolution 2005 Feb;59(2):464-468. [Medline]
- Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997 Sep 13;315(7109):629-634 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Duval S. The trim and fill method. In: Rothstein HR, Sutton AJ, Borenstein M, editors. Publication Bias in Meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons; 2005:127-144.
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Washington, DC: Author; 2013.
- Charlton JP, Danforth ID. Validating the distinction between computer addiction and engagement: online game playing and personality. Behav Inf Technol 2010 Nov;29(6):601-613. [CrossRef]
- Lemmens JS, Valkenburg PM, Peter J. Development and validation of a game addiction scale for adolescents. Media Psychol 2009 Mar 05;12(1):77-95. [CrossRef]
- Demetrovics Z, Urbán R, Nagygyörgy K, Farkas J, Griffiths MD, Pápay O, et al. The development of the Problematic Online Gaming Questionnaire (POGQ). PLoS One 2012 May 10;7(5):e36417 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Young KS. Internet Addiction: The Emergence of a New Clinical Disorder. In: CyberPsychology & Behavior. New York: Mary Ann Liebert, Inc; Jan 1998:237-244.
- Demetrovics Z, Urbán R, Nagygyörgy K, Farkas J, Zilahy D, Mervó B, et al. Why do you play? The development of the motives for online gaming questionnaire (MOGQ). Behav Res Methods 2011 Sep 13;43(3):814-825. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Yee N, Ducheneaut N, Nelson L. Online gaming motivations scale: development and validation. 2012 May Presented at: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; 2012 May; Austin, TX. [CrossRef]
- Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med 2002 Jun 15;21(11):1539-1558. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Sung H, Sigerson L, Cheng C. Social capital accumulation in location-based mobile game playing: a multiple-process approach. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw 2017 Aug;20(8):486-493. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Paavilainen J, Alha K, Korhonen H. A review of social features in social network games. ToDIGRA 2017 Sep 22;3(2):2. [CrossRef]
- Snodgrass JG, Zhao W, Lacy MG, Zhang S, Tate R. The cross-cultural expression of internet gaming distress in North America, Europe, and China. Addict Behav Rep 2019 Jun;9:100146 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Snodgrass JG, Zhao W, Lacy MG, Zhang S, Tate R. Distinguishing core from peripheral psychiatric symptoms: addictive and problematic internet gaming in North America, Europe, and China. Cult Med Psychiatry 2019 Jun;43(2):181-210. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- King DL, Delfabbro PH. Internet gaming disorder treatment: a review of definitions of diagnosis and treatment outcome. J Clin Psychol 2014 Oct 19;70(10):942-955. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Stevens MW, King DL, Dorstyn D, Delfabbro PH. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for internet gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Psychother 2019 Mar;26(2):191-203. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Torres-Rodríguez A, Griffiths MD, Carbonell X. The treatment of internet gaming disorder: a brief overview of the PIPATIC program. Int J Ment Health Addict 2018;16(4):1000-1015 [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Männikkö N, Billieux J, Nordström T, Koivisto K, Kääriäinen M. Problematic gaming behaviour in Finnish adolescents and young adults: relation to game genres, gaming motives and self-awareness of problematic use. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2016 Dec 27;15(2):324-338. [CrossRef]
- Maroney N, Williams BJ, Thomas A, Skues J, Moulding R. A stress-coping model of problem online video game use. Int J Ment Health Addiction 2018 Apr 16;17(4):845-858. [CrossRef]
- Scharkow M, Festl R, Vogelgesang J, Quandt T. Beyond the “core-gamer”: Genre preferences and gratifications in computer games. Comput Human Behav 2015 Mar;44:293-298. [CrossRef]
- Blinka L, Mikuška J. The role of social motivation and sociability of gamers in online game addiction. Cyberpsychology 2014 Jul 01;8(2):2. [CrossRef]
- Ko C, Yen J, Chen S, Wang P, Chen C, Yen C. Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. J Psychiatr Res 2014 Jun;53:103-110. [CrossRef] [Medline]
- Lee W. Understanding problematic use of massively multiplayer online games: instrument development and theoretical model testing. Hong Kong Baptist University. Hong Kong, China: Hong Kong Baptist University; 2015. URL: https://scholars.hkbu.edu.hk/ws/portalfiles/portal/55061425/OA-0186.pdf [accessed 2022-02-14]
- Gravdal L, Sandal GM. The two-factor model of social desirability: Relation to coping and defense, and implications for health. Pers Individ Dif 2006 Apr;40(5):1051-1061. [CrossRef]
Abbreviations
DSM-5: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders |
ICD-11: International Classification of Diseases, Eleventh Revision |
IGD: internet gaming disorder |
PRISMA: Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses |
Edited by J Torous; submitted 20.08.20; peer-reviewed by S Gunther, K Magnusson, Z Demetrovics; comments to author 17.11.20; revised version received 09.03.21; accepted 02.12.21; published 17.02.22
Copyright©Hsin-Yi Wang, Cecilia Cheng. Originally published in JMIR Mental Health (https://mental.jmir.org), 17.02.2022.
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Mental Health, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://mental.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.