Published on in Vol 11 (2024)

This is a member publication of National University of Singapore

Preprints (earlier versions) of this paper are available at https://preprints.jmir.org/preprint/54412, first published .
Examining the Effects of a Brief, Fully Self-Guided Mindfulness Ecological Momentary Intervention on Empathy and Theory-of-Mind for Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Randomized Controlled Trial

Examining the Effects of a Brief, Fully Self-Guided Mindfulness Ecological Momentary Intervention on Empathy and Theory-of-Mind for Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Randomized Controlled Trial

Examining the Effects of a Brief, Fully Self-Guided Mindfulness Ecological Momentary Intervention on Empathy and Theory-of-Mind for Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Randomized Controlled Trial

Authors of this article:

Nur Hani Zainal1, 2 Author Orcid Image ;   Michelle G Newman3 Author Orcid Image

Original Paper

1Department of Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

2Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore, Singapore, Singapore

3Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, United States

Corresponding Author:

Nur Hani Zainal, BS, MS, PhD

Department of Psychology

National University of Singapore

9 Arts Link

Singapore, 117572

Singapore

Phone: 65 917 767 7088

Fax:65 6516 3749

Email: hanizainal@nus.edu.sg


Background: The utility of brief mindfulness ecological momentary interventions (EMIs) to improve empathy and theory-of-mind has been underinvestigated, particularly in generalized anxiety disorder (GAD).

Objective: In this randomized controlled trial, we aimed to examine the efficacy of a 14-day, fully self-guided, mindfulness EMI on the empathy and theory-of-mind domains for GAD.

Methods: Adults (aged ≥18 y) diagnosed with GAD were randomized to a mindfulness EMI (68/110, 61.8%) or self-monitoring app (42/110, 38.2%) arm. They completed the Interpersonal Reactivity Index self-report empathy measure and theory-of-mind test (Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task) at prerandomization, postintervention, and 1-month follow-up (1MFU) time points. Hierarchical linear modeling was conducted with the intent-to-treat principle to determine prerandomization to postintervention (pre-post intervention) and prerandomization to 1MFU (pre-1MFU) changes, comparing the mindfulness EMI to self-monitoring.

Results: Observed effects were generally stronger from pre-1MFU than from pre-post intervention time points. From pre-post intervention time points, the mindfulness EMI was more efficacious than the self-monitoring app on fantasy (the ability to imagine being in others’ shoes; between-intervention effect size: Cohen d=0.26, P=.007; within-intervention effect size: Cohen d=0.22, P=.02 for the mindfulness EMI and Cohen d=−0.16, P=.10 for the self-monitoring app). From pre-1MFU time points, the mindfulness EMI, but not the self-monitoring app, improved theory-of-mind (a window into others’ thoughts and intentions through abstract, propositional knowledge about their mental states, encompassing the ability to decipher social cues) and the fantasy, personal distress (stress when witnessing others’ negative experiences), and perspective-taking (understanding others’ perspective) empathy domains. The effect sizes were small to moderate (Cohen d=0.15-0.36; P<.001 to P=.01) for significant between-intervention effects from pre-1MFU time points. Furthermore, the within-intervention effect sizes for these significant outcomes were stronger for the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.30-0.43; P<.001 to P=.03) than the self-monitoring app (Cohen d=−0.12 to 0.21; P=.001 to P>.99) from pre-1MFU time points. No between-intervention and within-intervention effects on empathic concern (feeling affection, compassion, and care when observing others in distress, primarily attending to their emotional well-being) were observed from pre-post intervention and pre-1MFU time points.

Conclusions: The brief mindfulness EMI improved specific domains of empathy (eg, fantasy, personal distress, and perspective-taking) and theory-of-mind with small to moderate effect sizes in persons with GAD. Higher-intensity, self-guided or coach-facilitated, multicomponent mindfulness EMIs targeting the optimization of social relationships are likely necessary to improve the empathic concern domain in this population.

Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT04846777; https://clinicaltrials.gov/study/NCT04846777

JMIR Ment Health 2024;11:e54412

doi:10.2196/54412

Keywords



Background

Empathy encompasses the capacity to comprehend and resonate with the emotional experiences of others, facilitating caregiving, knowledge sharing, and collaborative goal attainment [1,2]. Relatedly, theory-of-mind (ToM) offers insight into peoples’ thoughts and intentions via abstract, propositional knowledge concerning others’ mental states and encompasses the capacity to interpret social cues from others [3,4]. Both empathy and ToM play vital roles in comprehending the cognitive and emotional dynamics of others in social contexts [3]. Specifically, empathy and ToM have been linked to enhanced emotional well-being [5], stronger social connections [6], and improved social health [7]. Therefore, developing efficacious interventions to improve various empathy domains and ToM is essential.

In particular, it is possible that mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) could be efficacious in enhancing empathy and ToM skills [8]. Broadly, numerous theorists asserted that practicing mindfulness diligently should inherently give rise to interpersonal growth, including kindness, compassion, and empathy [9]. Plausibly, the receptive, nonjudgmental attitude fostered by mindfulness exercises could be pivotal in nurturing empathy toward others [10,11]. In addition, practicing mindfulness offers a conducive environment for efforts to comprehend another individual while acknowledging the inherent incompleteness of such understanding [12]. Collectively, mindfulness practices via MBIs can enhance individuals’ attunement to others’ circumstances, deepen their emotional resonance, increase their willingness to engage with negative emotions, and foster compassion toward others’ experiences as an extension of self-compassion [13].

Supporting these theories, a narrative systematic review provided compelling evidence for the positive impact of MBIs on enhancing empathy in children and adolescents [14]. Similarly, in an open trial, an 8-week mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) [15] among university students improved all empathy domains with moderate to large effects at pre-post intervention time points (Cohen d=0.48-1.19) [16]; however, the lack of a randomly assigned control intervention precluded the ability to make cause-effect inferences. Lending credence to this idea, meta-analytic data from experiments and randomized controlled trials (RCTs) indicated that MBIs were superior to control interventions in enhancing empathy in the healthy general population [17]. A particular case was how premedical and medical students’ participation in a mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) [18] program significantly elevated their overall empathy levels compared to those in the control intervention among premedical and medical students [19]. Another study showed that a 3-month perspective-taking–focused MBI led to greater ToM performance compared to an emotion-focused MBI [20]. Similarly, a recent pilot trial suggested the promise of MBIs in enhancing ToM in individuals with psychotic disorders [21]. Overall, the literature alludes to the high likelihood of efficacy of MBIs in enhancing empathy and ToM domains for various populations.

However, most of these MBIs, such as the popular 8-week MBCT and MBSR programs, necessitated in-person weekly individual or group intervention sessions lasting 60 to 150 minutes with day-long, 6-hour meditation retreats [22]. Thus, evaluating the impact of brief MBIs is crucial in light of the growing popularity of concise MBIs, such as brief smartphone-delivered mindfulness ecological momentary interventions (EMIs) and web-based audio streams [23]. Scant yet positive evidence for the possibility of brief mindfulness EMIs’ efficacy on empathy-related outcomes exists across 2 studies. First, a 5-minute mindfulness induction enhanced both ToM and empathic concern among meditation-naive adults more than among control adults [24]. Second, an 11-minute MBI versus relaxation led to enhanced out-intervention altruism while adjusting for levels of in-intervention empathy in the US general population [25]. Despite that, to the best of our awareness, there is no evidence yet that these effects endured beyond these mindfulness inductions, emphasizing the need to assess the efficacy of brief mindfulness EMIs on longer-term outcomes.

In addition, a significant limitation in the current literature on the efficacy of MBIs on social-cognitive outcomes is the focus on healthy samples despite ample evidence demonstrating empathy and ToM problems in populations with psychiatric disorders, such as depression [26], eating disorders [27], and anxiety disorders [28]. In recent years, there has been growing recognition of dysregulation in social processes, particularly cognitive and emotional empathy, as significant transdiagnostic contributors to internalizing disorders such as major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) in terms of their etiology, diagnostic relevance, and maintenance [29,30]. A meta-analysis showed that increased anxiety symptoms (including excessive worry in GAD) correlated with reduced perspective-taking and heightened the ability to be deeply immersed in imaginative fictional worlds (or fantasy) [31]. In addition, higher anxiety symptoms were associated with less pronounced empathic concern for others' emotional well-being and others’ personal distress, the inclination to absorb or experience stress in reaction to others’ feelings. These relationships imply that suboptimal empathy and ToM processes could be a common factor across various forms of anxiety disorders. Nonetheless, basic science [31,32] and clinical research [33] attention on this topic has been more heavily weighted toward social anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder [34], and posttraumatic stress disorder [35] than other anxiety disorders, such as GAD. This lacuna in the literature highlights the importance of investigating this topic in other anxiety disorders to inform novel treatment optimization efforts for GAD, a needed avenue that lacks exploration [36].

On that note, GAD, for which chronic excessive and uncontrollable worry features as a core symptom, is an understudied yet essential case in point. More specifically, individuals with GAD tended to have above-average ToM scores, but only for negative social cues when instructed to worry instead of relax [37]. In another study, those with GAD scored lower on a ToM assessment than non-GAD control intervention participants [38]. Furthermore, people with clinical depression and anxiety experienced increased worry symptoms on days when they pursued self-image goals above the sample average but encountered reduced worry symptoms when they pursued empathic, compassionate goals [39]. Broadly, persistent interpersonal challenges, including problems associated with empathy and social cognition, among a sizable subset of clients with GAD were linked to reduced progress both immediately after cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and during follow-up [40,41]. Altogether, these findings suggest that empathy and associated social-cognitive issues may be key maintenance factors in GAD. For these reasons, intensive psychotherapies integrating various theoretical modalities have been developed to enhance empathy in GAD [42], but their lengthiness and rigor preclude scalability. Collectively, suboptimal empathy, ToM, and interpersonal issues [42], coupled with a considerable reluctance to seek face-to-face mental health treatment in GAD [43], highlight the importance of determining whether brief mindfulness EMIs might be efficacious in targeting these social-cognitive outcomes for GAD.

This study was a secondary analysis of a published RCT, which showed that a brief mindfulness EMI reduced repetitive negative thinking and GAD severity and enhanced trait mindfulness and executive functioning among persons diagnosed with GAD [44]. Similar to this study, in prior research MBIs produced modest to moderate impacts on anxiety and depression symptoms [45], such as pathological worry [44,46-48]. In addition, a meta-analysis documented the substantial impact of MBIs on executive functioning in both nonclinical and clinical populations [49], such as persons with GAD [44]. Therefore, it is possible that a brief mindfulness EMI could help enhance ToM and empathy in those with GAD.

Objective

We aimed to examine how a brief mindfulness EMI, compared to a self-monitoring app, might improve empathy and ToM in GAD. On the basis of the theories and evidence outlined, we hypothesized that a brief mindfulness EMI would be more efficacious than a self-monitoring app in improving 4 established empathy domains [50] and ToM [51] from pre-post time points. The empathy domains included empathic concern (feeling affection, compassion, and genuine care when observing others’ distress, with attentiveness centered on their emotional well-being); fantasy (the capacity for immersive engagement in fictional scenarios through imagination); personal distress (the anxiety and stress individuals experience when observing the adversity of others); and perspective-taking (understanding others’ vantage point) [52]. Moreover, we expected such improvements to occur in the longer term from prerandomization to 1-month follow-up (1MFU) time points.


Ethical Considerations

This study was approved by the Pennsylvania State University Institutional Review Board (approval STUDY00010664). Participants were compensated up to US $30, subject pool credit hours, or a mixture of both. We preregistered the RCT on ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT04846777) and the hypotheses of this study on the Open Science Framework [53]. This study was conducted in compliance with the American Psychological Association and Declaration of Helsinki ethical standards in treating human participants. Informed consent was obtained from participants as per the Penn State Institutional Review Board. All data were de-identified.

Study Design

We used a 2 (intervention: mindfulness EMI; control: self-monitoring app) × 3 (time: prerandomization, postintervention, and 1MFU) mixed methods design to assess the efficacy of the mindfulness EMI versus self-monitoring app on each distinct empathy and ToM outcome. Intervention was the between-subject factor, whereas time was the within-subject factor. We recruited 110 participants, with 61.8% (68/110) in the mindfulness EMI arm and 38.2% (42/110) in the self-monitoring app arm. Multimedia Appendix 1 [54-66] provides a comprehensive overview of the study’s methodology, including power analysis and reimbursement. Figure 1 displays the CONSORT (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) diagram [67,68], illustrating participant enrollment and progression (refer to Multimedia Appendix 2 for the CONSORT-EHEALTH [Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials of Electronic and Mobile Health Applications and Online Telehealth] checklist).

Figure 1. CONSORT (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) flowchart of participant recruitment and progress. GAD: generalized anxiety disorder; GAD-Q-IV: Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire, Fourth Version; EMI: ecological momentary intervention.

Eligibility Criteria

Included participants were required to meet diagnostic criteria for GAD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) [54] and the GAD Questionnaire, Fourth Version (GAD-Q-IV) [55]. Initially, prospective participants underwent screening with the GAD-Q-IV. Subsequently, we invited those who scored at or above the clinical threshold for a brief clinical interview. The Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule (ADIS) for DSM-5 [54,56] was used to establish their psychiatric diagnoses. Furthermore, participants were required to be aged at least 18 years, possess either an iPhone or Android phone, and provide informed consent. Exclusion criteria encompassed the presence of suicidal thoughts, manic episodes, disorders of psychosis, or substance use disorders.

Participants

We recruited help-seeking participants diagnosed with GAD, who were not currently under the care of a mental health professional, from the local community and psychology subject pool. Table 1 displays the sociodemographic characteristics of the participants. Furthermore, the prevalence of comorbid diagnoses (eg, current or recurrent major depressive disorder, panic disorder, social anxiety disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, alcohol use disorder, substance use disorder, anorexia nervosa, and binge-eating disorder) at baseline did not significantly differ between arms (all P>.05).

Table 1. Sociodemographic data of study participants in the mindfulness EMIa and self-monitoring app arms (N=110).
Sociodemographic characteristicsMindfulness EMI arm (n=68)Self-monitoring app arm (n=42)P value
Continuous variables, mean (SD)

Age (y)20.53 (3.91)21.24 (7.24).51

14-item GAD-Q-IVb score9.52 (2.10)9.94 (1.96).30

Treatment expectations


Credibility6.00 (1.39)5.72 (1.58).34


Expectancy43.46 (17.33)44.29 (18.13).31
Categorical variables, n (%)

Gender orientation.85


Women10 (14.7)5 (11.9)


Men57 (83.8)37 (88.1)


Declined to disclose1 (1.5)N/Ac

Race.99


African American5 (7.4)1 (2.4)


Asian or Asian American11 (16.2)4 (9.5)


Declined to disclose1 (1.5)0 (0)


Hispanic3 (4.4)5 (11.9)


Other race4 (5.9)2 (4.8)


White44 (64.7)27 (64.3)

Comorbid diagnoses


Current major depressive episode32 (47.1)24 (57.1).30


Recurrent major depressive episode25 (36.8)20 (47.6).26


Current panic disorder16 (23.5)5 (11.9).13


Current social anxiety disorder15 (22.1)14 (33.3).19


Current OCDd4 (5.9)4 (9.5).48


Current PTSDe9 (13.2)4 (9.5).56


Current alcohol use disorder7 (10.3)1 (2.4).12


Current substance use disorder3 (4.4)1 (2.4).58


Current anorexia nervosa0 (0)0 (0)N/A


Current binge-eating disorder1 (1.5)0 (0).39

aEMI: ecological momentary intervention.

bGAD-Q-IV: Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire, Fourth Edition.

cN/A: not applicable.

dOCD: obsessive compulsive disorder.

ePTSD: posttraumatic stress disorder.

Prerandomization Diagnostic Interview and Screening Assessment

Mental Disorder Diagnoses

The ADIS-5 [56] was used as a semistructured interview based on the DSM-5 criteria [69]. Every ADIS-5 interview, whether face-to-face or via Zoom (Zoom Video Communications), was video-recorded and meticulously conducted by highly trained undergraduate and Bachelor of Arts–level assessors. All assessors had watched standardized training videos and completed quality assurance tests to maximize implementation fidelity to the study protocol. The study protocol was implemented remotely via Zoom during the COVID-19 pandemic. A subset comprising 40% (45/110) of these video recordings underwent a secondary evaluation by an independent, unbiased rater. We assessed the interrater reliability for GAD diagnoses. Any discrepancies were addressed through discussions and eventual consensus. The interrater agreement for GAD diagnosis was outstanding (Cohen κ=1.00), whereas, for other comorbid diagnoses and rule-outs, it ranged from very good to outstanding (average Cohen κ=0.75-0.98).

GAD Diagnosis

The screening for GAD used the 14-item GAD-Q-IV [55], which included a combination of dichotomous responses (“yes” or “no”) and continuous response formats (9-point Likert scales for items assessing the level of distress and interference attributed to GAD symptoms).

Intervention Arms
Mindfulness EMI App

During the first visit, assessors either exited the physical room (before the pandemic) or directed participants to turn off their Zoom audio and video before accessing the Qualtrics (Qualtrics International Inc) link to play the relevant intervention video (during the pandemic). In the mindfulness EMI, an instructional video featuring the principal investigator (a PhD-level clinical psychologist) was presented, delivering critical principles of evidence-based MBI protocols akin to the principles in MBSR [70]. Participants assigned to this condition were introduced to a precise conceptualization of mindfulness, with an explicit directive to immerse themselves entirely in their present circumstances and be engaged in activities. This segment was designed to endow individuals with chronic worry tendencies with proficiency in open monitoring, facilitating their capacity to attend to minute details. Following this, the video therapist taught techniques for deliberate, unhurried, rhythmic diaphragmatic breathing retraining, followed by a demonstration of its proper implementation. This element encompassed instruction in techniques for cultivating tranquility through controlled respiratory training and the cultivation of mindful qualities, such as observance without reactivity or judgment, drawing from the principles of MBCT [15]. Next, the video therapist underscored the significance and advantages of integrating mindfulness into daily routines. Subsequently, all assessors uniformly administered the 6-item Credibility and Expectancy Questionnaire [57]. Concluding this phase, assessors attentively addressed any inquiries about procedural but not the intervention aspects of the study (Multimedia Appendix 3). Participants were given a mindfulness EMI handout sent web-based in an automated manner via Qualtrics to preserve assessor blinding. The handout explicitly instructed them to review and practice its contents regularly.

The EMI encouraged individuals to engage in mindfulness exercises daily, precisely at 5 distinct intervals throughout the day: approximately 9 AM, noon, 3 PM, 6 PM, and 9 PM, over a span of 2 weeks. Within every instance of engagement with the mindfulness EMI, individuals participating in the program were provided with the following standard guidance instructions (Multimedia Appendices 1 and 3):

Pay attention to your breathing. Breathe in a slow, steady, and rhythmic manner. Stay focused on the sensations of the air coming into your lungs and then letting it out. As you are breathing, observe your experience as it is. Let go of judgments that do not serve you. Focus on the here and now. Attend to the small moments right now (e.g., reading a chapter, having a cool glass of water), as that is where enjoyment, peace, and serenity in life happen.

Individuals assessed their current levels of mindfulness (“To what extent are you experiencing the present moment fully?”), depression (“To what degree do you feel depressed right now?”), and anxiety (“To what degree do you feel keyed up or on edge right now?”) before and after receiving these instructions on a 9-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (extremely). Every mindfulness EMI notification concluded with the following motivating message to foster the enduring adoption of these skills:

Remember that the cultivation of mindfulness is lifelong. The goal of therapy is to be your own therapist. Practice mindfulness between the prompts and after you have completed this study.
Self-Monitoring App

In the self-monitoring app, the standardized video commenced with the principal investigator delineating self-monitoring as the heightened awareness of cognitive processes and emotional states. Subsequently, the video advanced by positing that the mere act of vigilantly tracking one’s thoughts and documenting any associated emotional distress could promote the cultivation of more adaptive cognitive patterns. Finally, the self-monitoring video conveyed the notion that the act of self-monitoring, in and of itself, possessed the potential to ameliorate feelings of anxiety. The foundational rationale for the self-monitoring condition was derived and adapted from the rationale used in a recent, brief app intervention [71]. This approach was crafted to closely mimic the mindfulness EMI protocol while excluding its hypothesized active, helpful components: acceptance, diaphragmatic breathing retraining, awareness toward subtle experiences, open monitoring, and sustained mindfulness practice. Consequently, the self-monitoring app deliberately abstained from any reference to the concept of mindfulness. This approach refrained from imparting explicit directives for participants to heighten their sensitivity and consciousness of their ongoing experiences; instead, its emphasis rested on vigilant monitoring of their thoughts and emotional responses. Moreover, participants were not tasked with focusing exclusively on their immediate activities because such a directive could inadvertently induce emotional state alterations. Although self-monitoring participants were directed to observe their cognitions and emotional states, there was a deliberate omission of instructions about accepting these thoughts and feelings as they manifested. Furthermore, the intervention did not include any guidance regarding breathing retraining techniques. It was not designed to elicit any sensations associated with relaxation typically linked to abdominal breathing. Participants were not instructed to engage in self-monitoring activities between the prescribed prompts or beyond the conclusion of the initial 2-week period. Consequently, self-monitoring, unlike mindfulness, was delimited to the active intervention period. The self-monitoring approach was also designed to control for credibility and expectancy effects, pre-empting regression to the mean and averting potential inflation of effect sizes that typically happen with a no-treatment or waitlist control (Multimedia Appendix 4).

In contrast to the more extensive mindfulness guidance provided by the mindfulness EMI, participants in the self-monitoring intervention received concise instructions 5 times a day (approximately 9 AM, noon, 3 PM, 6 PM, and 9 PM) over a span of 14 days (Multimedia Appendices 1 and 4): “Notice your thoughts and how distressing they may be.” We assessed individuals’ levels of mindfulness, depression, and anxiety using identical 9-point Likert scale questions both before and after each prompt in the self-monitoring sessions. Next, mirroring the mindfulness EMI, all assessors administered the 6-item Credibility and Expectancy Questionnaire. Furthermore, participants were provided a copy of the self-monitoring handout in an automated manner programmed via Qualtrics. Unlike the mindfulness EMI handout, this handout did not provide explicit instructions to review its contents routinely. In addition, the self-monitoring app was chosen as a placebo control as previous theory and research suggested that it would not be strong enough to elicit improvements in empathy and related social cognition relative to an MBI [72] yet could serve adequately as a placebo in an RCT [73].

Prerandomization, Postintervention, and 1MFU Measures

Overview

Table 2 summarizes descriptive statistics of the empathy domains and ToM scores.

Table 2. Descriptive data of empathy and ToMa variables across prerandomization, postintervention, and 1MFUb time points in the mindfulness EMIc (n=68) and self-monitoring (n=42) app arms (N=110).

Values, mean (SE)dSkewnessKurtosis
Mindfulness EMI at the prerandomizationtime point

ToM (BLERTe)17.85 (0.78)−1.050.12

Perspective taking (IRIf)3.66 (0.28)−0.22−0.77

Fantasy (IRI)3.53 (0.29)−0.27−1.05

Empathic concern (IRI)3.97 (0.26)−0.960.72

Personal distress (IRI)3.13 (0.27)−0.16−0.88
Self-monitoringat the prerandomizationtime point

ToM (BLERT)17.81 (0.34)−0.07−1.23

Perspective taking (IRI)3.62 (0.12)−0.11−1.25

Fantasy (IRI)3.71 (0.13)−0.38−0.04

Empathic concern (IRI)3.93 (0.12)−0.35−0.69

Personal distress (IRI)3.08 (0.12)0.17−0.79
Mindfulness EMI at the postinterventiontime point

ToM (BLERT)17.94 (1.66)−1.122.46

Perspective taking (IRI)3.68 (0.66)−0.23−0.94

Fantasy (IRI)3.87 (0.68)−0.65−0.83

Empathic concern (IRI)4.10 (0.60)−1.311.71

Personal distress (IRI)3.21 (0.63)−0.10−1.10
Self-monitoring at the postinterventiontime point

ToM (BLERT)17.95 (0.73)−1.575.33

Perspective taking (IRI)3.60 (0.29)0.14−0.81

Fantasy (IRI)3.45 (0.30)−0.57−0.31

Empathic concern (IRI)3.74 (0.26)−0.46−0.41

Personal distress (IRI)2.80 (0.28)0.01−0.13
Mindfulness EMI at the 1MFUtime point

ToM (BLERT)19.13 (1.54)−1.122.46

Perspective taking (IRI)3.90 (0.56)−0.23−0.94

Fantasy (IRI)3.85 (0.57)−0.65−0.83

Empathic concern (IRI)4.11 (0.51)−1.311.71

Personal distress (IRI)3.38 (0.57)−0.10−1.10
Self-monitoring at the 1MFUtime point

ToM (BLERT)17.98 (0.68)−0.04−1.05

Perspective taking (IRI)3.62 (0.25)−0.31−0.77

Fantasy (IRI)3.57 (0.25)−0.09−0.43

Empathic concern (IRI)3.80 (0.22)−0.57−0.21

Personal distress (IRI)2.71 (0.25)−0.11−0.29

aToM: theory-of-mind.

b1MFU: 1-month follow-up time point.

cEMI: ecological momentary intervention.

dThese marginal means and SEs were derived from the hierarchical linear models.

eBLERT: Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task.

fIRI: Interpersonal Reactivity Index.

Empathy Domains

We used the 28-item Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [50] to assess 4 trait-level empathy domains, each with a subscale [74]. Empathic concern refers to experiencing affection and compassion when witnessing others’ distress, focusing on the well-being of others’ feelings (eg, “I am often quite touched by things that I see happen.”). Fantasy pertains to peoples’ imaginative ability to immerse themselves in fictional scenarios (eg, “I really get involved with the feelings of the characters in a novel.”). Personal distress refers to individuals’ feelings of anxiety and stress when witnessing others’ negative experiences (eg, “In emergency situations, I feel apprehensive and ill-at-ease.”). Perspective-taking entails comprehending others’ viewpoint (eg, “I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things look from their perspective.”). Each subscale comprised 4 items rated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Possible values of each subscale range from 1 to 5. The internal consistency (Cronbach α) of the IRI empathic concern scale was .85, .88, and .89 at prerandomization, postintervention, and 1MFU time points (fantasy: .82, .89, and .90; personal distress: .81, .90, and .89; and perspective tasking: .85, .88, and .89), respectively. Each IRI subscale has shown high convergent validity, acceptable discriminant validity [75], strong retest reliability, and good cross-cultural generalizability [76].

ToM Domains

We evaluated ToM using the Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task (BLERT) [51]. BLERT comprises 21 brief video clips where an actor enacts one of 3 dialogue options while expressing 7 distinct emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise, and no emotion). Participants selected the emotion that best corresponded to the emotion expressed by the actor from the 7 options displayed. An overall accuracy score was calculated (based on a score of 1 for each correct trial). Possible values range from 0 to 21. Cronbach α values of the BLERT total scale were .94, .89, and .95 at prerandomization, postintervention, and 1MFU time points, respectively. Furthermore, a prior review showed that the BLERT had high retest reliability, strong convergent validity, and good discriminant validity [77].

Procedures

At visit 1, participants initially underwent the structured ADIS-5 interview. Afterward, eligible individuals completed a battery of prerandomization self-reports and behavioral and neuropsychological assessments in a manner that was counterbalanced, thus minimizing any potential for order-related biases. Assessors were uninformed regarding assigned arms, ensuring that the treatment assignment remained concealed from them throughout all study visits. They either left the physical room (before the COVID-19 pandemic) or provided clear instructions to participants to deactivate their Zoom audio and video before activating the Qualtrics link to play the pertinent treatment video (amid the COVID-19 pandemic). Participants installed the Personal Analytics Companion app, preloaded with either the mindfulness EMI or self-monitoring tool, onto their respective smartphones. The assessor was present if participants had questions about study procedures (eg, future study visits and technical questions about installing the app on their phone) but was not present when the participants were notified about their assigned intervention arm and its components. Participants were told that they would receive prompts at 5 designated times each day, at approximately 9 AM, noon, 3 PM, 6 PM, and 9 PM, over the ensuing 14 days, and these prompts could be flexibly adjusted based on their schedules. The prompts provided specific instructions to guide them to use mindfulness or self-monitoring strategies, contingent upon their assigned intervention. After 14 days, all participants reconvened at the laboratory (or on Zoom) for postintervention assessments and again during the 1MFU time point, during which they completed the requisite self-reports and neuropsychological battery. Participants received compensation in credit hours, monetary remuneration, or a combination of both (Multimedia Appendix 1). In addition, the research team performed a compliance verification on the seventh day and extended invitations to those participants who successfully passed this compliance assessment to continue in the treatment.

Data Analyses

All analyses were based on the intent-to-treat principle. Of the 110 participants assigned to the mindfulness EMI (n=68, 61.8%) or self-monitoring app (n=42, 38.2%) arm, 98 (89.1%) participants conscientiously finished engaging with the 6-week study protocol, including all study visit assessments and ≥ 80% of the app prompts. A total of 12 (10.7%) out of 110 participants did not meet the seventh-day engagement threshold by failing to complete at least 80% of the EMI prompts from both arms over the 2-week intervention period. However, the data of these 12 (10.7%) participants were included in all analyses. The median number of prompts completed was 63 (range 0-70). To address missing values (29/1650, 1.76% of the entire data set), we conducted multiple imputation using chained equations with the predictive mean matching algorithm under the missing at-random assumption [78]. Specifically, we synthesized the data from 100 imputed data sets, each with a maximum of 10 iterations [79]. As another preprocessing step, we determined from a series of ordinary least square regressions that there were no significant between-intervention effects on BLERT ToM (β=.04, P=.92), IRI empathic concern (β=.05, P=.74), IRI fantasy (β=−.18, P=.26), IRI personal distress (β=.05, P=.74), and IRI perspective-taking (β=.04, P=.80) domains at prerandomization (baseline) time point. Furthermore, there were no significant differences in perceived credibility or expectancy between the interventions, with Cohen d values ranging from −0.05 to 0.19. Therefore, no covariates were included in the hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) predictive equations, given randomization success (Table 1).

HLM was used to compare interventions over time on all empathy-related outcomes. HLM considers the nesting of time points within participants, enabling the exploration of changes within and between participants over time (prerandomization, postintervention, and 1MFU time points) and by intervention (mindfulness EMI and self-monitoring app) [80,81]. Distinct models were used for each of the 5 outcomes. To assess efficacy of the mindfulness EMI versus self-monitoring app, we analyzed the time at level 1, delineating the first segment from pre-post intervention time points and a second segment from prerandomization to 1MFU (pre-1MFU) time points. The third segment from postintervention to 1MFU (post-1MFU) time points examined maintenance of any treatment gains. In each HLM model, fixed-effect predictors included intervention, time, and their interaction. The random-effect predictor was the intercept, which was coded for time (eg, prerandomization time point as “0” and postintervention time point as “1”), allowing for variability in average outcome values among participants. We used fitted models to calculate estimated mean scores at each time point. Cohen d effect sizes and their corresponding 95% CIs were computed to facilitate the interpretation of parameter estimates [82-84]. In this context, Cohen d values of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 represent small, moderate, and large effects, respectively.


ToM: Propositional Knowledge of Others’ Emotional and Mental States

Despite nonsignificant intervention differences from pre-post intervention time points (Cohen d=−0.01, 95% CI −0.21 to 0.19; P=.91), the mindfulness EMI (vs the self-monitoring app) led to significantly greater change in ToM (BLERT scores) from pre-1MFU (Cohen d=0.25, 95% CI 0.05-0.45; P=.01; Table 3). From pre-post intervention, no significant changes in ToM occurred in either the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.03, 95% CI −0.17 to 0.23; P=.75) or self-monitoring (Cohen d=0.03, 95% CI −0.16 to 0.23; P=.74) interventions. From pre-1MFU, the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.43, 95% CI 0.23-0.63; P<.001) but not the self-monitoring app (Cohen d=0.06, 95% CI −0.14 to 0.25; P=.56) significantly enhanced ToM (Table 4). No substantial between-intervention and within-intervention effects on ToM emerged from post-1MFU time points (Tables S1 and S2 in Multimedia Appendix 5).

Table 3. Hierarchical linear models of between-intervention mindfulness ecological momentary intervention versus self-monitoring app effects on empathy and ToMa variables from prerandomization to postintervention (pre-post intervention) and prerandomization to 1-month follow-up (pre-1MFU) time points.

Pre-post intervention time pointsPre-1MFU time points

βbP valueCohen d (95% CI)βP valueCohen d (95% CI)
ToM (BLERTc)

Intercept17.81<.0014.94 (4.56 to 5.33)17.81<.0014.94 (4.56 to 5.33)

Time0.14.710.04 (−0.16 to 0.23)0.08.620.05 (−0.15 to 0.24)

Intervention0.04.920.01 (−0.19 to 0.21)0.04.920.01 (−0.19 to 0.21)

Time × intervention−0.05.91−0.01 (−0.21 to 0.19)0.56.010.25 (0.05 to 0.45)
Empathic concern (IRId)

Intercept3.93<.0013.24 (2.95 to 3.53)3.93<.0013.24 (2.95 to 3.53)

Time−0.18.23−0.12 (−0.31 to 0.08)−0.06.25−0.11 (−0.31 to 0.09)

Intervention0.05.740.03 (−0.16 to 0.23)0.05.740.03 (−0.16 to 0.23)

Time × intervention0.30.110.15 (−0.04 to 0.35)0.13.060.18 (−0.02 to 0.38)
Fantasy (IRI)

Intercept3.71<.0012.78 (2.51 to 3.05)3.71<.0012.78 (2.51 to 3.05)

Time−0.26.13−0.14 (−0.34 to 0.05)−0.07.25−0.11 (−0.31 to 0.09)

Intervention−0.18.26−0.11 (−0.3 to 0.09)−0.18.26−0.11 (−0.3 to 0.09)

Time × intervention0.60.0070.26 (0.06 to 0.46)0.23.0040.28 (0.09 to 0.48)
Personal distress (IRI)

Intercept3.08<.0012.46 (2.20 to 2.71)3.08<.0012.46 (2.20 to 2.71)

Time−0.28.08−0.17 (−0.37 to 0.03)−0.18.005−0.27 (−0.47 to −0.08)

Intervention0.05.740.03 (−0.16 to 0.23)0.05.740.03 (−0.16 to 0.23)

Time × intervention0.36.0830.17 (−0.03 to 0.36)0.31<.0010.36 (0.16 to 0.56)
Perspective taking (IRI)

Intercept3.62<.0012.77 (2.50 to 3.04)3.62<.0012.77 (2.50 to 3.04)

Time−0.02.89−0.01 (−0.21 to 0.18)0.00.990.00 (−0.20 to 0.20)

Intervention0.04.800.02 (−0.17 to 0.22)0.04.800.02 (−0.17 to 0.22)

Time × intervention0.04.840.02 (−0.18 to 0.22)0.12.130.15 (−0.05 to 0.34)

aToM: theory-of-mind.

bβ: regression unstandardized parameter estimate.

cBLERT: Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task.

dIRI: Interpersonal Reactivity Index.

Table 4. Hierarchical linear models of within-intervention mindfulness EMIa and self-monitoring app effects on empathy and ToMb variables from prerandomization to postintervention (pre-post intervention) and prerandomization to 1-month follow-up (pre-1MFU) time points.

Pre-post intervention time pointsPre-1MFU time points

βcP valueCohen d (95% CI)βP valueCohen d (95% CI)
ToM (BLERTd)

Intercept (mindfulness EMI)17.85<.0015.50 (5.08 to 5.92)17.85<.0015.50 (5.08 to 5.92)

Time (mindfulness EMI)0.09.750.03 (−0.17 to 0.23)0.64<.0010.43 (0.23 to 0.63)

Intercept (self-monitoring)17.81<.0017.10 (6.58 to 7.62)17.81<.0017.10 (6.58 to 7.62)

Time (self-monitoring)0.14.740.03 (−0.16 to 0.23)0.08.560.06 (−0.14 to 0.25)
Empathic concern (IRIe)

Intercept (mindfulness EMI)3.97<.0014.29 (3.94 to 4.64)3.97<.0014.29 (3.94 to 4.64)

Time (mindfulness EMI)0.12.260.11 (−0.09 to 0.31)0.07.060.18 (−0.01 to 0.38)

Intercept (self-monitoring)3.93<.0013.11 (2.82 to 3.40)3.93<.0013.11 (2.82 to 3.40)

Time (self-monitoring)−0.18.28−0.11 (−0.30 to 0.09)−0.06.35−0.09 (−0.29 to 0.11)
Fantasy (IRI)

Intercept (mindfulness EMI)3.53<.0013.08 (2.80 to 3.37)3.53<.0013.08 (2.80 to 3.37)

Time (mindfulness EMI)0.34.020.22 (0.03 to 0.42)0.16.0020.30 (0.11 to 0.5)

Intercept (self-monitoring)3.71<.0013.36 (3.06 to 3.66)3.71<.0013.36 (3.06 to 3.66)

Time (self-monitoring)−0.26.10−0.16 (−0.36 to 0.03)−0.07.22−0.12 (−0.31 to 0.08)
Personal distress (IRI)

Intercept (mindfulness EMI)3.13<.0013.00 (2.72 to 3.28)3.13<.0013.00 (2.72 to 3.28)

Time (mindfulness EMI)0.07.600.05 (−0.15 to 0.25)0.12.030.21 (0.02 to 0.41)

Intercept (self-monitoring)3.08<.0012.75 (2.48 to 3.02)3.08<.0012.75 (2.48 to 3.02)

Time (self-monitoring)−0.28.03−0.21 (−0.41 to −0.02)−0.18.001−0.33 (−0.53 to −0.13)
Perspective taking (IRI)

Intercept (mindfulness EMI)3.66<.0013.43 (3.13 to 3.74)3.66<.0013.43 (3.13 to 3.74)

Time (mindfulness EMI)0.02.890.01 (−0.18 to 0.21)0.12.010.25 (0.06 to 0.45)

Intercept (self-monitoring)3.62<.0012.95 (2.67 to 3.23)3.62<.0012.95 (2.67 to 3.23)

Time (self-monitoring)−0.02.89−0.01 (−0.21 to 0.18)0.00.990.00 (−0.20 to 0.20)

aEMI: ecological momentary intervention.

bToM: theory-of-mind.

cβ: unstandardized regression parameter estimate.

dBLERT: Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task.

eIRI: Interpersonal Reactivity Index.

Empathic Concern: Care About Others’ Psychological Well-Being

There was no significant difference between the mindfulness EMI and self-monitoring interventions in effects on empathic concern (IRI scores) from pre-post intervention (Cohen d=0.15, 95% CI −0.04 to 0.35; P=.11) and pre-1MFU (Cohen d=0.18, 95% CI −0.02 to 0.38; P=.06; Table 3) time points. From pre-post intervention, there were no significant changes in empathic concern in either the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.11, 95% CI −0.09 to 0.31; P=.26) or self-monitoring (Cohen d=0.11, 95% CI −0.30 to 0.09; P=.28) interventions. Similarly, from pre-1MFU, no significant changes in empathic concern emerged in the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.18, 95% CI −0.01 to 0.38; P=.06) or self-monitoring (Cohen d=−0.09, 95% CI −0.29 to 0.11; P=.35; Table 4) interventions. No significant between-intervention and within-intervention effects on empathic concern emerged from post-1MFU time points (Tables S1 and S2 in Multimedia Appendix 5).

Fantasy: the Ability to Imagine Others’ Experiences

The mindfulness EMI (vs self-monitoring app) led to greater effects on fantasy (IRI scores) from pre-post intervention (Cohen d=0.26, 95% CI 0.06-0.46; P=.007) and pre-1MFU (Cohen d=0.28, 95% CI 0.09-0.48; P=.004; Table 3). From pre-post intervention, the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.22, 95% CI 0.03-0.42; P=.02) but not the self-monitoring app (Cohen d=−0.16, 95% CI −0.36 to 0.03; P=.10) generated significant improvement in fantasy. Similarly, from pre-1MFU, the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.30, 95% CI 0.11-0.50; P=.002) but not the self-monitoring app (Cohen d=−0.12, 95% CI −0.31 to 0.08; P=.22) significantly enhanced fantasy (Table 4). Maintenance of gains occurred from post-1MFU time points (Tables S1 and S2 in Multimedia Appendix 5).

Personal Distress: Feeling Distress When Observing Others' Adverse Experiences

There were no significant intervention differences In personal distress (IRI scores) from pre-post intervention (Cohen d=0.17, 95% CI −0.03 to 0.36; P=.08). At the same time, from pre-post intervention, the self-monitoring app (Cohen d=−0.21, 95% CI −0.41 to −0.02; P=.03) but not the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.05, 95% CI −0.15 to 0.25; P=.60) significantly reduced personal distress. There were significant differences between mindfulness EMI and self-monitoring interventions from pre-1MFU time points (Cohen d=0.36, 95% CI 0.16-0.56; P<.001; Table 3). Although the mindfulness EMI significantly increased personal distress (Cohen d=0.21, 95% CI 0.02-0.41; P=.03), the self-monitoring app significantly decreased it (Cohen d=−0.33, 95% CI −0.53 to −0.13; P=.001; Table 4). No significant between-intervention and within-intervention effects on personal distress emerged from post-1MFU time points (Tables S1 and S2 in Multimedia Appendix 5).

Perspective Taking: Comprehending Others’ Viewpoint

There were no significant differences between mindfulness EMI and self-monitoring on perspective-taking (IRI scores) from pre-post intervention (Cohen d=0.02, 95% CI −0.18 to 0.22; P=.84) and pre-1MFU (Cohen d=0.15, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.34; P=.13; Table 3). From pre-post intervention, no significant changes in perspective-taking emerged in the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.01, 95% CI −0.18 to 0.21; P=.89) and self-monitoring (Cohen d=−0.01, 95% CI −0.21 to 0.18; P=.89) interventions. However, from pre-1MFU, the mindfulness EMI (Cohen d=0.25, 95% CI 0.06-0.45; P=.01) but not the self-monitoring app (Cohen d=0.00, 95% CI −0.20 to 0.20; P=.99; Table 4) yielded significant improvements in perspective-taking. No significant between-intervention and within-intervention effects on perspective taking emerged from post-1MFU time points (Tables S1 and S2 in Multimedia Appendix 5).


Principal Findings and Comparison With Prior Work

Partially supporting our hypothesis, brief self-guided mindfulness EMI displayed longer-term efficacy from pre-1MFU time points compared to self-monitoring in improving 3 of the 5 examined social-cognitive domains: ToM, the capacity for imaginative immersion in various scenarios (fantasy), and experiencing distress when observing others’ adverse situations (personal distress). Moreover, the mindfulness EMI, but not self-monitoring, enhanced perspective-taking from pre-1MFU time points despite the lack of between-intervention effects. An unexpected outcome was that neither of the interventions yielded effects on empathic concern. Our findings cannot be attributed to sociodemographic and comorbid psychiatric diagnoses, as those variables did not differ between compared conditions at prerandomization. On the whole, these outcomes suggest specificity, instead of globality, in the impact of brief mindfulness EMI and self-monitoring on ToM and trait-level empathy domains in the context of GAD. Potential theories are put forth to advance clinical science on this underinvestigated yet essential topic.

Why did brief mindfulness EMI but not self-monitoring enhance ToM (the ability to interpret social cues and represent abstract propositional knowledge about others’ emotional and mental states), the capacity to envision being in another person’s shoes (fantasy), and perspective-taking across 6 weeks? These outcomes extended reports of positive correlations between mindfulness and the ability to understand the emotions of self and others cognitively (cognitive empathy) [85-87]. The facets of nonjudgmental contemplation and a focus on the present moment emphasized by the mindfulness EMI could wield significant influence in cultivating empathic responses. Such mindfulness EMI instructions might augment cognitive capabilities, such as adopting another’s perspective [9] and fostering a pragmatic understanding of human distress [88]. Collectively, our findings and these testable interpretations aligned harmoniously with previous 8- to 12-week MBI RCTs [89,90], highlighting the benefits of the mindfulness EMI’s brevity on these social-cognitive domains.

Moreover, although the brief mindfulness EMI improved negative affective empathy (distress) or anxiety and stress experienced when confronted with the negative experiences of others, self-monitoring worsened it from pre-1MFU time points. This finding was inconsistent with a study showing that a brief mindfulness EMI was associated with increased cognitive empathy but decreased affective empathy (the capacity to share in the emotional experiences of others vicariously) [91]. Perhaps the brief mindfulness EMI evaluated in this study might have safeguarded against desensitization to suffering or developing callousness, enabling one to witness others’ adverse experiences without feeling overwhelmed [92]. The opposite pattern occurred in the self-monitoring intervention, probably due to instructions to focus inwardly on one’s stressful feelings and thoughts without asking the participant to have regard for others’ difficulties.

Contrary to expectations, the brief mindfulness EMI did not outperform the self-monitoring app in improving empathic concern (experiencing affection and compassion when witnessing others’ distress). Furthermore, no changes in empathic concern occurred from pre-post intervention and pre-1MFU time points. Similarly, a prior experiment found that a 5-minute mindfulness induction, compared to 2 control conditions, did not yield any discernible impact on empathic responses [93]. On the basis of prior RCTs that tested empirically-supported 8-week MBIs [94,95], a tenable account is a need for lengthier, higher-intensity (fully self-guided or coach-guided), multicomponent mindfulness EMIs focused on interpersonal relationships to evoke measurable improvement in empathic concern [96,97]. Future studies could test this conjecture and evaluate dose-response relationships between mindfulness EMI duration or intensity and its impact on various empathy domains. Alternatively, as prior research showed that people diagnosed with GAD experienced notably above-average levels of empathic concern [98], perhaps other empathy domains apart from empathic concern were more malleable to change via mindfulness EMIs in this population. A future RCT that recruited participants with both GAD and below-average levels of empathic concern would provide a direct test of this hypothesis.

As far as we know, there has been no other study that examined the effects of MBI on empathy in individuals with GAD. One prior study found that CBT but not MBI led to changes in positive affective empathy in individuals with social anxiety disorder compared to a waitlist control condition [33]. Furthermore, a change in positive affective empathy was a mediator of change in CBT but not in MBSR [33]. Given that we found changes in empathy and ToM domains relative to a control condition, perhaps enhancements in ToM and empathy domains are mechanisms of change of the effects of MBIs on GAD symptom alteration and related outcomes but not on social anxiety disorder. Conducting a mediation analysis to determine if changes in empathy and ToM domains were potential mechanisms of how brief mindfulness EMIs might improve GAD symptoms was beyond the scope of this study. However, a future report using the same data set will aim to explore this possibility.

Limitations and Strengths

Interpreting the findings within the context of the limitations herein is imperative for an in-depth understanding. First, it is worth noting that the 2-week intervention duration might be inadequate to elicit immediate enhancements in empathy domains; nevertheless, the results showed greater promise for habitual worriers from pre-1MFU time points. Second, we did not incorporate assessments to gauge mindfulness EMI participants’ ongoing mindfulness skill use during the post-1MFU evaluation. Future investigations should, therefore, test the potential impact of continuous mindfulness practices, even in the absence of recurring guidance via the mindfulness EMI, on any discernible between-intervention effects during follow-up assessments. Third, we only assessed ToM and 4 empathy domains; extensive theories have suggested that MBIs are helpful in increasing other social-cognitive outcomes, such as altruism and prosocial behaviors [99], and these theories should be explored in GAD and related disorders. On that note, clinical science can profit from examining the effect of mindfulness EMIs on multimodal measures of empathy, such as proinflammatory cytokines, oxytocin, and neuroimaging indexes [100,101]. Finally, recruiting more diverse samples is required to establish cross-cultural generalizability [102,103].

Nonetheless, this study demonstrated notable strengths, including the gold standard RCT design featuring an active control intervention, a commendably high compliance rate, the recruitment of a clinically diagnosed sample with ample statistical power, and the incorporation of comprehensive assessments. Furthermore, it is noteworthy that our attrition rate, standing at a mere 11%, fell below the typical 24% to 50% range observed in RCTs delivered through smartphones [104,105]. Our relatively minimal attrition may be attributed to the prorated reimbursement schedule design.

Conclusions

In summary, these findings suggest that brief self-guided mindfulness EMIs such as ours hold promise as scalable solutions to enhance specific empathy domains (perspective-taking and fantasy or the ability to immerse oneself imaginatively in diverse situations experienced by others), personal distress (feeling distressed when witnessing unfavorable circumstances affecting others), and ToM (the skill to decipher social signals) for persons with GAD. Nonetheless, mindfulness EMI did not elicit between-intervention and within-intervention effects on empathic concern (experiencing warmth, compassion, and sincere concern while observing another person’s distress) for this population. To advance our understanding of the genuine therapeutic efficacy of mindfulness EMIs (brief or prolonged) on social-cognitive domains, it becomes crucial to discern the specific subinterventions for whom it benefits, the contexts in which it exerts its effects, and when it may be contraindicated or inadvisable. Consequently, these efforts might address global challenges to social integration, anxiety disorders, and other psychological well-being aspects encountered by communities.

Acknowledgments

This trial received partial funding from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH115128), the Penn State Office of Research and Graduate Studies Dissertation Award, the Penn State Susan Welch or Arthur Nagle Family Graduate Fellowship, the National University of Singapore Development Grant, and the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies Leonard Krasner Student Dissertation Award.

NHZ’s research assistants (Ally Atkinson, Ana Luiza Clever Galvao, Drishti Sanghvi, Emma Win, Emily Forcht, Jamie Gensbauer, Kate Miller, Madeline Noelle Lamonica, Madeleine Miller, Marietta Kocher, Natalie Gottret, Natalie Suzette Marr, and Vivian Vo Nguyen) assisted heavily with the data collection and various logistical aspects of this study.

Authors' Contributions

All authors have (1) made substantial contributions to the analysis and interpretation of the study and its findings, (2) drafted and revised the paper for intellectual content, and (3) given their final approval of the version to be submitted. The manuscript has been read and approved by all authors. NHZ was involved in the conceptualization, methodology (design and creation of models), funding acquisition, formal analysis, coding with the R software (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) for data analysis, visualization, and writing (original draft, reviewing, and editing) of this work. MGN assisted with the manuscript’s conceptualization, study design, data curation, validation, supervision, and writing (original draft, reviewing, and editing). NHZ and MGN take full responsibility for the data, accuracy of analyses and interpretation, and conduct of the research.

Conflicts of Interest

None declared.

Multimedia Appendix 1

Study methodology details.

DOCX File , 45 KB

Multimedia Appendix 2

CONSORT E-HEALTH checklist.

PDF File (Adobe PDF File), 391 KB

Multimedia Appendix 3

Screenshots of the intervention arm.

DOCX File , 1349 KB

Multimedia Appendix 4

Screenshots of the control arm.

DOCX File , 722 KB

Multimedia Appendix 5

Postintervention to 1-month follow-up outcomes.

DOCX File , 26 KB

  1. de Waal FB, Preston SD. Mammalian empathy: behavioural manifestations and neural basis. Nat Rev Neurosci. Aug 2017;18(8):498-509. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  2. Preston SD, de Waal FB. Only the PAM explains the personalized nature of empathy. Nat Rev Neurosci. Dec 2017;18(12):769. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  3. Preckel K, Kanske P, Singer T. On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind. Curr Opin Behav Sci. Feb 2018;19:1-6. [CrossRef]
  4. Schurz M, Radua J, Tholen MG, Maliske L, Margulies DS, Mars RB, et al. Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: a neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychol Bull. Mar 2021;147(3):293-327. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  5. Morelli SA, Lieberman MD, Zaki J. The emerging study of positive empathy. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. Feb 05, 2015;9(2):57-68. [CrossRef]
  6. Morelli SA, Ong DC, Makati R, Jackson MO, Zaki J. Empathy and well-being correlate with centrality in different social networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. Sep 12, 2017;114(37):9843-9847. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  7. Zaki J. Integrating empathy and interpersonal emotion regulation. Annu Rev Psychol. Jan 04, 2020;71:517-540. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  8. Bayot M, Vermeulen N, Kever A, Mikolajczak M. Mindfulness and empathy: differential effects of explicit and implicit Buddhist teachings. Mindfulness. Jun 2, 2018;11(1):5-17. [CrossRef]
  9. Block-Lerner J, Adair C, Plumb JC, Rhatigan DL, Orsillo SM. The case for mindfulness-based approaches in the cultivation of empathy: does nonjudgmental, present-moment awareness increase capacity for perspective-taking and empathic concern? J Marital Fam Ther. Oct 2007;33(4):501-516. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  10. Malin Y. Others in mind: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between mindfulness and prosociality. Mindfulness. Jun 13, 2023;14(7):1582-1605. [CrossRef]
  11. Feldman C, Kuyken W. Compassion in the landscape of suffering. Contemporary Buddhism. Jun 14, 2011;12(1):143-155. [CrossRef]
  12. Cooper D, Yap K, O’Brien M, Scott I. Mindfulness and empathy among counseling and psychotherapy professionals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mindfulness. Jun 08, 2020;11(10):2243-2257. [CrossRef]
  13. Bruce NG, Shapiro SL, Constantino MJ, Manber R. Psychotherapist mindfulness and the psychotherapy process. Psychotherapy (Chic). Mar 2010;47(1):83-97. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  14. Cheang R, Gillions A, Sparkes E. Do mindfulness-based interventions increase empathy and compassion in children and adolescents: a systematic review. J Child Fam Stud. Apr 20, 2019;28(7):1765-1779. [CrossRef]
  15. Segal ZJ, Williams MG, Teasdale JD. Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression: A New Approach to Preventing Relapse. New York, NY. The Guilford Press; 2002.
  16. Centeno RP. Effect of mindfulness on empathy and self-compassion: an adapted MBCT program on Filipino college students. Behav Sci (Basel). Feb 27, 2020;10(3):61. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  17. Hu Z, Wen Y, Wang Y, Lin Y, Shi J, Yu Z, et al. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based interventions on empathy: a meta-analysis. Front Psychol. 2022;13:992575. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  18. Kabat-Zinn J. An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. Apr 1982;4(1):33-47. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  19. Shapiro SL, Schwartz GE, Bonner G. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical students. J Behav Med. Dec 1998;21(6):581-599. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  20. Trautwein F, Kanske P, Böckler A, Singer T. Differential benefits of mental training types for attention, compassion, and theory of mind. Cognition. Jan 2020;194:104039. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  21. López-Navarro E, Fonseca-Pedrero E, Errasti J, Al-Halabí S. Mindfulness improves theory of mind in people experiencing psychosis: a pilot randomized clinical trial. Psychiatry Res. Apr 2022;310:114440. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  22. Creswell JD. Mindfulness Interventions. Annu Rev Psychol. Jan 03, 2017;68:491-516. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  23. Wu J, Ma Y, Zuo Y, Zheng K, Zhou Z, Qin Y, et al. Effects of mindfulness exercise guided by a smartphone app on negative emotions and stress in non-clinical populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Public Health. 2021;9:773296. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  24. Tan LB, Lo BC, Macrae CN. Brief mindfulness meditation improves mental state attribution and empathizing. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e110510. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  25. Zheng D, Berry DR, Brown KW. Effects of brief mindfulness meditation and compassion meditation on parochial empathy and prosocial behavior toward ethnic out-group members. Mindfulness (N Y). Mar 27, 2023:1-17. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  26. Schreiter S, Pijnenborg GH, Aan Het Rot M. Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. J Affect Disord. Aug 15, 2013;150(1):1-16. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  27. Kerr-Gaffney J, Harrison A, Tchanturia K. Cognitive and affective empathy in eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:102. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  28. Baez S, Tangarife MA, Davila-Mejia G, Trujillo-Güiza M, Forero DA. Performance in emotion recognition and theory of mind tasks in social anxiety and generalized anxiety disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. 2023;14:1192683. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  29. Xu X, Dai J, Liu C, Chen Y, Xin F, Zhou F, et al. Common and disorder-specific neurofunctional markers of dysregulated empathic reactivity in major depression and generalized anxiety disorder. Psychother Psychosom. 2020;89(2):114-116. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  30. Tone EB, Tully EC. Empathy as a "risky strength": a multilevel examination of empathy and risk for internalizing disorders. Dev Psychopathol. Nov 2014;26(4 Pt 2):1547-1565. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  31. Nair TK, Waslin SM, Rodrigues GA, Datta S, Moore MT, Brumariu LE. A meta-analytic review of the relations between anxiety and empathy. J Anxiety Disord. Jan 2024;101:102795. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  32. Pittelkow M, Aan Het Rot M, Seidel LJ, Feyel N, Roest AM. Social anxiety and empathy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Anxiety Disord. Mar 2021;78:102357. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  33. Morrison AS, Mateen MA, Brozovich FA, Zaki J, Goldin PR, Heimberg RG, et al. Changes in empathy mediate the effects of cognitive-behavioral group therapy but not mindfulness-based stress reduction for social anxiety disorder. Behav Ther. Nov 2019;50(6):1098-1111. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  34. Bora E. Social cognition and empathy in adults with obsessive compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. Oct 2022;316:114752. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  35. Stevens JS, Jovanovic T. Role of social cognition in post-traumatic stress disorder: a review and meta-analysis. Genes Brain Behav. Jan 2019;18(1):e12518. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  36. Newman MG, Przeworski A. J Anxiety Disord. May 2018;56:11-13. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  37. Zainal NH, Newman MG. Worry amplifies theory-of-mind reasoning for negatively valenced social stimuli in generalized anxiety disorder. J Affect Disord. Feb 2018;227:824-833. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  38. Aydın O, Balıkçı K, Çökmüş FP, Ünal Aydın P. The evaluation of metacognitive beliefs and emotion recognition in panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder: effects on symptoms and comparison with healthy control. Nord J Psychiatry. 2019;73(4-5):293-301. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  39. Erickson TM, Granillo MT, Crocker J, Abelson JL, Reas HE, Quach CM. Compassionate and self-image goals as interpersonal maintenance factors in clinical depression and anxiety. J Clin Psychol. Apr 2018;74(4):608-625. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  40. Borkovec TD, Newman MG, Pincus AL, Lytle R. A component analysis of cognitive-behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder and the role of interpersonal problems. J Consult Clin Psychol. Sep 01, 2002;70(2):288-298. [CrossRef]
  41. Hara KM, Westra HA, Constantino MJ, Antony MM. The impact of resistance on empathy in CBT for generalized anxiety disorder. Psychother Res. Jul 2018;28(4):606-615. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  42. Watson J, Timulak L, Greenberg LS. Emotion-focused therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. In: Greenberg LS, Goldman RN, editors. Clinical Handbook of Emotion-Focused Therapy. New York, NY. American Psychological Association; 2019:315-336.
  43. Goetter EM, Frumkin MR, Palitz SA, Swee MB, Baker AW, Bui E, et al. Barriers to mental health treatment among individuals with social anxiety disorder and generalized anxiety disorder. Psychol Serv. Feb 2020;17(1):5-12. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  44. Zainal NH, Newman MG. A randomized controlled trial of a 14-day mindfulness ecological momentary intervention (MEMI) for generalized anxiety disorder. Eur Psychiatry. Jan 16, 2023;66(1):e12. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  45. Blanck P, Perleth S, Heidenreich T, Kröger P, Ditzen B, Bents H, et al. Effects of mindfulness exercises as stand-alone intervention on symptoms of anxiety and depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther. Mar 2018;102:25-35. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  46. de Abreu Costa M, D’Alò de Oliveira GS, Tatton-Ramos T, Manfro GG, Salum GA. Anxiety and stress-related disorders and mindfulness-based interventions: a systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis and meta-regression of multiple outcomes. Mindfulness. Nov 15, 2018;10(6):996-1005. [CrossRef]
  47. Ghahari S, Mohammadi-Hasel K, Malakouti SK, Roshanpajouh M. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for generalised anxiety disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. East Asian Arch Psychiatry. Jun 2020;30(2):52-56. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  48. Haller H, Breilmann P, Schröter M, Dobos G, Cramer H. A systematic review and meta-analysis of acceptance- and mindfulness-based interventions for DSM-5 anxiety disorders. Sci Rep. Oct 14, 2021;11(1):20385. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  49. Zainal NH, Newman MG. Mindfulness enhances cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis of 111 randomized controlled trials. Health Psychol Rev. Aug 30, 2023:1-27. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  50. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. Jan 1983;44(1):113-126. [CrossRef]
  51. Bell M, Bryson G, Lysaker P. Positive and negative affect recognition in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Res. Nov 14, 1997;73(1-2):73-82. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  52. Bonfils KA, Lysaker PH, Minor KS, Salyers MP. Empathy in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the interpersonal reactivity index. Psychiatry Res. Mar 2017;249:293-303. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  53. Zainal NH, Newman MG. Does a brief mindfulness ecological momentary intervention (MEMI) improve empathy and theory-of-mind for generalized anxiety disorder? A randomized controlled trial. Open Science Framework. 2023. URL: https://osf.io/vbsjd [accessed 2024-03-23]
  54. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edition. Arlington, VA. American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
  55. Newman MG, Zuellig AR, Kachin KE, Constantino MJ, Przeworski A, Erickson T, et al. Preliminary reliability and validity of the generalized anxiety disorder questionnaire-IV: a revised self-report diagnostic measure of generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Ther. 2002;33(2):215-233. [CrossRef]
  56. Brown TA, Barlow DH. Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-5 (ADIS-5L): Client Interview Schedule. Oxford, UK. Oxford University Press; 2014.
  57. Devilly GJ, Borkovec TD. Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. Jun 2000;31(2):73-86. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  58. Arend MG, Schäfer T. Statistical power in two-level models: a tutorial based on Monte Carlo simulation. Psychol Methods. Feb 2019;24(1):1-19. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  59. Homack S, Lee D, Riccio CA. Test review: delis-Kaplan executive function system. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. Jul 2005;27(5):599-609. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  60. powerlmm: power analysis for longitudinal multilevel models. The Comprehensive R Archive Network. URL: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/powerlmm/index.html [accessed 2024-04-05]
  61. Martínez-Loredo V, Fernández-Hermida JR, Carballo LS, Fernández-Artamendi S. Long-term reliability and stability of behavioral measures among adolescents: the Delay Discounting and Stroop tasks. J Adolesc. Jul 2017;58:33-39. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  62. Moore MT, Anderson NL, Barnes JM, Haigh EA, Fresco DM. Using the GAD-Q-IV to identify generalized anxiety disorder in psychiatric treatment seeking and primary care medical samples. J Anxiety Disord. Jan 2014;28(1):25-30. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  63. Rutter LA, Brown TA. Reliability and validity of the dimensional features of generalized anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. Jan 2015;29:1-6. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  64. Shankman SA, Funkhouser CJ, Klein DN, Davila J, Lerner D, Hee D. Reliability and validity of severity dimensions of psychopathology assessed using the structured clinical interview for DSM-5 (SCID). Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. Mar 2018;27(1):e1590. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  65. Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Kroenke K, Linzer M, deGruy FV, Hahn SR, et al. Utility of a new procedure for diagnosing mental disorders in primary care. The PRIME-MD 1000 study. JAMA. Dec 14, 1994;272(22):1749-1756. [Medline]
  66. Wade EC, Cohen RT, Loftus P, Ruscio AM. A novel measure of real-time perseverative thought. Clin Psychol Sci. May 2022;10(3):534-552. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  67. Boutron I, Altman DG, Moher D, Schulz KF, Ravaud P, CONSORT NPT Group. CONSORT statement for randomized trials of nonpharmacologic treatments: a 2017 update and a CONSORT extension for nonpharmacologic trial abstracts. Ann Intern Med. Jul 04, 2017;167(1):40-47. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  68. Eysenbach G, CONSORT-EHEALTH Group. CONSORT-EHEALTH: improving and standardizing evaluation reports of Web-based and mobile health interventions. J Med Internet Res. Dec 31, 2011;13(4):e126. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  69. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic And Statistical Manual Of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition, Text Revision (DSM-5-TR). New York, NY. American Psychiatric Association; 2022.
  70. Kabat-Zinn J. Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain and Illness. New York, NY. Delacorte Press; 1990.
  71. LaFreniere LS, Newman MG. A brief ecological momentary intervention for generalized anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial of the worry outcome journal. Depress Anxiety. Sep 2016;33(9):829-839. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  72. Clarke J, Proudfoot J, Whitton A, Birch MR, Boyd M, Parker G, et al. Therapeutic alliance with a fully automated mobile phone and web-based intervention: secondary analysis of a randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment Health. Feb 25, 2016;3(1):e10. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  73. Lutz J, Offidani E, Taraboanta L, Lakhan SE, Campellone TR. Appropriate controls for digital therapeutic clinical trials: a narrative review of control conditions in clinical trials of digital therapeutics (DTx) deploying psychosocial, cognitive, or behavioral content. Front Digit Health. Aug 18, 2022;4:823977. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  74. Chrysikou EG, Thompson WJ. Assessing cognitive and affective empathy through the interpersonal reactivity index: an argument against a two-factor model. Assessment. Dec 2016;23(6):769-777. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  75. Bonfils KA, Longenecker JM, Hammer LA, Luther L. Measuring empathy in groups with high schizotypy: psychometric evaluation of the interpersonal reactivity index. Psychol Assess. May 2022;34(5):459-466. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  76. Fernández AM, Dufey M, Kramp U. Testing the psychometric properties of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) in Chile. Eur J Psychol Assess. Jan 2011;27(3):179-185. [CrossRef]
  77. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Harvey PD. Social cognition psychometric evaluation: results of the initial psychometric study. Schizophr Bull. Mar 2016;42(2):494-504. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  78. Hughes RA, Heron J, Sterne JA, Tilling K. Accounting for missing data in statistical analyses: multiple imputation is not always the answer. Int J Epidemiol. Aug 01, 2019;48(4):1294-1304. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  79. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM. Multiple imputation using chained equations: issues and guidance for practice. Stat Med. Feb 20, 2011;30(4):377-399. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  80. Myers ND, Brincks AM, Ames AJ, Prado GJ, Penedo FJ, Benedict C. Multilevel modeling in psychosomatic medicine research. Psychosom Med. 2012;74(9):925-936. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  81. Faraway JJ. Extending the Linear Model with R: Generalized Linear, Mixed Effects and Nonparametric Regression Models 2nd edition. Boca Raton, FL. CRC Press; 2016.
  82. Feingold A. A regression framework for effect size assessments in longitudinal modeling of group differences. Rev Gen Psychol. Mar 2013;17(1):111-121. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  83. Feingold A. Confidence interval estimation for standardized effect sizes in multilevel and latent growth modeling. J Consult Clin Psychol. Feb 2015;83(1):157-168. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  84. Feingold A. Meta-analysis with standardized effect sizes from multilevel and latent growth models. J Consult Clin Psychol. Mar 2017;85(3):262-266. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  85. Beitel M, Ferrer E, Cecero JJ. Psychological mindedness and awareness of self and others. J Clin Psychol. Jun 2005;61(6):739-750. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  86. Fulton CL, Cashwell CS. Mindfulness‐based awareness and compassion: predictors of counselor empathy and anxiety. Couns Educ Superv. Jun 02, 2015;54(2):122-133. [CrossRef]
  87. Thomas JT, Otis MD. Intrapsychic correlates of professional quality of life: mindfulness, empathy, and emotional separation. J Soc Social Work Res. Jan 2010;1(2):83-98. [CrossRef]
  88. Raab K. Mindfulness, self-compassion, and empathy among health care professionals: a review of the literature. J Health Care Chaplain. Jun 13, 2014;20(3):95-108. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  89. Chan SH, Yu CK, Li AW. Impact of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on counseling self-efficacy: a randomized controlled crossover trial. Patient Educ Couns. Feb 2021;104(2):360-368. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  90. Schonert-Reichl KA, Oberle E, Lawlor MS, Abbott D, Thomson K, Oberlander TF, et al. Enhancing cognitive and social-emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: a randomized controlled trial. Dev Psychol. Jan 2015;51(1):52-66. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  91. Borghi O, Mayrhofer L, Voracek M, Tran US. Differential associations of the two higher-order factors of mindfulness with trait empathy and the mediating role of emotional awareness. Sci Rep. Feb 24, 2023;13(1):3201. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  92. Leonard HD, Campbell K, Gonzalez VM. The relationships among clinician self-report of empathy, mindfulness, and therapeutic alliance. Mindfulness. Mar 12, 2018;9(6):1837-1844. [CrossRef]
  93. Ridderinkhof A, de Bruin EI, Brummelman E, Bögels SM. Does mindfulness meditation increase empathy? An experiment. Self Identity. Jan 02, 2017;16(3):251-269. [CrossRef]
  94. Shapiro SL, Brown KW, Thoresen C, Plante TG. The moderation of mindfulness-based stress reduction effects by trait mindfulness: results from a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychol. Mar 2011;67(3):267-277. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  95. Wallmark E, Safarzadeh K, Daukantaitė D, Maddux RE. Promoting altruism through meditation: an 8-week randomized controlled pilot study. Mindfulness. Jun 4, 2012;4(3):223-234. [CrossRef]
  96. Goldberg SB, Imhoff-Smith T, Bolt DM, Wilson-Mendenhall CD, Dahl CJ, Davidson RJ, et al. Testing the efficacy of a multicomponent, self-guided, smartphone-based meditation app: three-armed randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment Health. Nov 27, 2020;7(11):e23825. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  97. Silveira S, Godara M, Singer T. Boosting empathy and compassion through mindfulness-based and socioemotional dyadic practice: randomized controlled trial with app-delivered trainings. J Med Internet Res. Jul 26, 2023;25:e45027. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  98. Cordaro M, Grigsby TJ, Howard JT, Deason RG, Haskard-Zolnierek K, Howard K. Pandemic-specific factors related to generalized anxiety disorder during the initial COVID-19 protocols in the United States. Issues Ment Health Nurs. Aug 2021;42(8):747-757. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  99. Van Doesum NJ, Van Lange DA, Van Lange PA. Social mindfulness: skill and will to navigate the social world. J Pers Soc Psychol. Jul 2013;105(1):86-103. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  100. Trofimova IN, Gaykalova AA. Emotionality vs. other biobehavioural traits: a look at neurochemical biomarkers for their differentiation. Front Psychol. 2021;12:781631. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  101. Weisz E, Zaki J. Motivated empathy: a social neuroscience perspective. Curr Opin Psychol. Dec 2018;24:67-71. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  102. Zainal NH, Newman MG, Hong RY. Cross-cultural and gender invariance of transdiagnostic processes in the United States and Singapore. Assessment. Mar 2021;28(2):485-502. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  103. Van Doren N, Zainal NH, Newman MG. Cross-cultural and gender invariance of emotion regulation in the United States and India. J Affect Disord. Dec 01, 2021;295:1360-1370. [FREE Full text] [CrossRef] [Medline]
  104. Linardon J, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M. Attrition and adherence in smartphone-delivered interventions for mental health problems: a systematic and meta-analytic review. J Consult Clin Psychol. Jan 2020;88(1):1-13. [CrossRef] [Medline]
  105. Torous J, Lipschitz J, Ng M, Firth J. Dropout rates in clinical trials of smartphone apps for depressive symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. Feb 15, 2020;263:413-419. [CrossRef] [Medline]


1MFU: 1-month follow-up
ADIS: Anxiety and Related Disorders Interview Schedule
BLERT: Bell-Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task
CBT: cognitive behavioral therapy
CONSORT: Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials
CONSORT-EHEALTH: Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials of Electronic and Mobile Health Applications and Online Telehealth
DSM-5: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fifth Edition
EMI: ecological momentary intervention
GAD: generalized anxiety disorder
GAD-Q-IV: Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire, Fourth Version
HLM: hierarchical linear modeling
IRI: Interpersonal Reactivity Index
MBCT: mindfulness-based cognitive therapy
MBI: mindfulness-based intervention
MBSR: mindfulness-based stress reduction
RCT: randomized controlled trial
ToM: theory-of-mind


Edited by J Torous; submitted 08.11.23; peer-reviewed by E Bernstein, N Van Doren; comments to author 23.11.23; revised version received 26.12.23; accepted 09.01.24; published 24.05.24.

Copyright

©Nur Hani Zainal, Michelle G Newman. Originally published in JMIR Mental Health (https://mental.jmir.org), 24.05.2024.

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work, first published in JMIR Mental Health, is properly cited. The complete bibliographic information, a link to the original publication on https://mental.jmir.org/, as well as this copyright and license information must be included.